首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2068篇
  免费   96篇
  2023年   11篇
  2021年   13篇
  2020年   32篇
  2019年   41篇
  2018年   38篇
  2017年   47篇
  2016年   70篇
  2015年   41篇
  2014年   43篇
  2013年   262篇
  2012年   88篇
  2011年   89篇
  2010年   58篇
  2009年   65篇
  2008年   66篇
  2007年   97篇
  2006年   75篇
  2005年   77篇
  2004年   78篇
  2003年   78篇
  2002年   74篇
  2001年   30篇
  2000年   38篇
  1999年   37篇
  1998年   30篇
  1997年   30篇
  1996年   33篇
  1995年   42篇
  1994年   33篇
  1993年   25篇
  1992年   27篇
  1991年   24篇
  1990年   23篇
  1989年   22篇
  1988年   17篇
  1987年   18篇
  1986年   15篇
  1985年   13篇
  1984年   18篇
  1983年   25篇
  1982年   33篇
  1981年   16篇
  1980年   24篇
  1979年   17篇
  1978年   17篇
  1977年   18篇
  1976年   13篇
  1975年   15篇
  1974年   16篇
  1973年   9篇
排序方式: 共有2164条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
921.
The cerebral lateralization pattern for speech production in normal hearing and congenitally deaf children was studied using the dual-task paradigm. Performance under the verbal task conditions showed predicted left hemispheric dominance for speech production in the normal hearing children. No developmental trends in asymmetry were found, suggesting that speech lateralization is present in normal 3-year-old children. These data support the developmental invariance hypothesis of cerebral organization. Deaf children showed more symmetrical patterns of cerebral control for speech production. No developmental trends in functional brain organization were observed among prepubescent deaf children.  相似文献   
922.
Latencies were measured using a modified four-choice reaction time (4-CRT) test procedure; the responses comprised forward and backward displacements of left- and right-hand levers. Two stimuli were presented, separated by an interval (ISI) of 25, 165, 415 or 815 msec. S1 designated which response should be readied; and on 62% of occasions, S2 indicated that the prepared response should be executed immediately (same trials). During transition trials, S2 specified which one of the alternative responses should be substituted. Same conditions produced significantly shorter reaction times. Transition latencies varied depending on the modification required: changing direction proved easier than changing either hand or hand and direction. This is compatible with a response coding strategy in which limb is designated before direction. Latencies and exchange function analyses that preparations were discontinued after response selection; even at the longest ISI, there was no convincing evidence of preparatory motor programming.  相似文献   
923.
924.
925.
926.
927.
Garry  Ann 《Philosophical Studies》1998,89(2-3):375-385
Philosophical Studies -  相似文献   
928.
The present study focused on the association between the particular sexual qualities midlife women desired in themselves and their husbands and the changes in sexual response they were experiencing. In a U.S. sample of 280 women participating in the ongoing Midlife Women's Health Survey, 40% reported a change in their sexual response, more often as decrements than increases, although desiring more nongenital touching was frequently noted. Respondents wanted more change in their own than in their partners' sexual qualities. In addition, the desire to change sexual qualities in oneself and in one's husband was significantly related to many changes in sexual responsivity. These findings are discussed in the context of a woman-defined sexuality at midlife.  相似文献   
929.
This study examined relations of multiple indicators of work identity and family identity with the number of weekly hours worked by 193 married business professionals. We found that men generally worked long hours regardless of the situational demands to work long hours and the strength of their work and family identities. Women's work hours, on the other hand, were associated with their work and family identities when weak situational demands permitted discretion over their work hours. We suggest that these sex differences can be explained by the ways in which women and men construe their work and family identities.  相似文献   
930.
Presents an obituary for John M. Neale. Neale died in Hilton Head, South Carolina, on November 19, 2011, after a long illness. He was born on August 31, 1943, in Toronto, Canada. He received his bachelor's degree from the University of Toronto in 1965, where his interest in psychology had been sparked by an introductory course taught by George Mandler. After working at a residential treatment center for emotionally disturbed children, he decided to pursue graduate training in clinical psychology and enrolled at Vanderbilt University. Rue Cromwell served as John's mentor and stimulated his interest in the investigation of perception and cognition in schizophrenia. His doctorate was awarded in 1969, after completion of his internship at the Langley Porter Neuropsychiatric Institute in San Francisco. John was hired in 1969 as an assistant professor in the new and exciting psychology department (founded in 1965) at the State University of New York at Stony Brook. That department remained his academic home for his entire career. Outside of his academic pursuits, John was an avid New York Giants fan, an extensive traveler, an excellent skier and tennis player, a music lover and jukebox collector, an outstanding cook, a terrific dancer, and a devoted dog owner. He continued to pursue these interests throughout his life, taking cooking classes, traveling to exotic locales with his wife Gail, and, when his health precluded more rigorous athletic pursuits, faithfully walking and playing with his dogs. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved).  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号