全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1886篇 |
免费 | 26篇 |
专业分类
1912篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 8篇 |
2021年 | 13篇 |
2020年 | 30篇 |
2019年 | 41篇 |
2018年 | 30篇 |
2017年 | 41篇 |
2016年 | 67篇 |
2015年 | 38篇 |
2014年 | 39篇 |
2013年 | 234篇 |
2012年 | 76篇 |
2011年 | 81篇 |
2010年 | 50篇 |
2009年 | 53篇 |
2008年 | 54篇 |
2007年 | 87篇 |
2006年 | 67篇 |
2005年 | 69篇 |
2004年 | 68篇 |
2003年 | 65篇 |
2002年 | 65篇 |
2001年 | 25篇 |
2000年 | 32篇 |
1999年 | 34篇 |
1998年 | 27篇 |
1997年 | 29篇 |
1996年 | 32篇 |
1995年 | 42篇 |
1994年 | 34篇 |
1993年 | 24篇 |
1992年 | 23篇 |
1991年 | 17篇 |
1990年 | 22篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 14篇 |
1986年 | 13篇 |
1985年 | 12篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 22篇 |
1982年 | 31篇 |
1981年 | 17篇 |
1980年 | 21篇 |
1979年 | 13篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 11篇 |
1975年 | 14篇 |
1974年 | 15篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有1912条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
981.
Disclosure of HIV status is an important topic for youth living with HIV/AIDS, yet theoretical frameworks for understanding HIV disclosure motivations have been poorly applied. Self-determination theory (SDT) proposes that people are at optimal functioning when they are engaging in activities that are interesting and enlivening. This study utilized SDT to understand young adults’ motivations to disclose their HIV status. Interviews and observations were conducted with nine youth aged 17–19 and two adult staff. Results indicate that SDT is useful for understanding types of motivation (i.e., amotivation, controlled, and autonomous motivation) to disclose. Amotivation was the most common type of motivation, and came from two recursive sources: fear of stigma and previous experiences of others disclosing without their consent. Controlled motivation to disclose occurred when participants were motivated to disclose because of reasons related to other people, rather than internal or personal reasons, and included the reasons of wanting to gain a closer relationship, reciprocate a shared secret, for psychological or emotional relief, and for attention. Autonomous motivation included two themes: the life perspective that “Having HIV is just part of who I am,” and valuing educating others because education was perceived as important and beneficial to others. This study extends SDT into the domain of HIV disclosure in older adolescents. People providing guidance and support to older adolescents with HIV/AIDS can use SDT to understand different motivations to disclose. 相似文献
982.
Mary Lynn Damhorst John M. Littrell Mary Ann Littrell 《The Journal of psychology》2013,147(6):553-562
The purpose of this study was to explore differences in body satisfaction among female adolescents 14 to 18 years of age. Principal components analysis of a body cathexis measure completed by 751 high school cheerleaders indicated that six factors defined the adolescents' attitudes toward their bodies: satisfaction with the midsection and overall appearance, peripheral body parts, hair and face, mouth, hands, and height. An Age x Body Satisfaction multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted; a Bonferroni adjusted alpha of .007 was established for subsequent ANOVAs. Satisfaction with the mouth increased significantly as age increased, and a similar but nonsignificant trend was found for peripheral body parts. The findings contradict previous research reporting no age differences in the body satisfaction of adolescents. Role demands and physical maturation of the cheerleaders may explain differences based on age. 相似文献
983.
984.
Abstract As a way for practitioners to integrate various models of marital therapy into their treatment,a flow chart of the process of psychotherapy with couples is presented. Basic steps and questions to be addressed are sequentially discussed. Resolution of each stage is considered important for movement to following stages. The flow chart, as a device for conceptualizing clients independent of a theoretical approach, allows for intervention strategies to be developed at each process level 相似文献
985.
Kimberly Cuevas Kirby Deater‐Deckard Jungmeen Kim‐Spoon Zhe Wang Katherine C. Morasch Martha Ann Bell 《The British journal of developmental psychology》2014,32(1):50-64
Despite the importance of executive function (EF) in both clinical and educational contexts, the aetiology of individual differences in early childhood EF remains poorly understood. This study provides the first longitudinal intergenerational analysis of mother–child EF associations during early childhood. A group of children and their mothers (n = 62) completed age‐appropriate EF tasks. Mother and child EFs were modestly correlated by 24 months of age, and this association was stable through 48 months. Importantly, maternal–child EF associations were still robust after controlling for verbal ability (potential indicator of verbal/crystallized intelligence) and maternal education (correlate of socio‐economic status and verbal intelligence). Potential implications of these findings as well as underlying mechanisms of the maternal–child EF association (gene–environment interplay) are discussed. 相似文献
986.
A theory of education requires a theory of the mind that is to be educated. Modules are the central feature of the mind of the infant. Innate learning devices, modules, prepare the infant with the following competences: language, number, theory of mind, spatial navigation, music, etc. We propose that the child be taught what evolution has prepared the child to learn. This program requires adding both modular—e.g., Theory of Mind, morality, art—as well as domain-general competences—e.g., analogies, causal analysis—to the curriculum. The program further requires revamping the presently inappropriate teaching of certain modular competences. Finally it requires correcting “evolutionary errors”—modular competences that were not ideal (see Galileo); or that, though once beneficial, have become harmful in the modern world (see imitation). This proposal is taken from [Premack, D. & Premack, A. (2003). Original intelligence. New York: Macgraw-Hill]. 相似文献
987.
Education has long been charged with the taskof forming and shaping subjectivity andidentity. However, the prevailing view ofeducation as a project of producing rationalautonomous subjects has been challenged bypostmodern and poststructuralist critiques ofsubstantial subjectivity. In a similar vein,Emmanuel Levinas inverts the traditionalconception of subjectivity, claiming that weare constituted as subjects only in respondingto the other. In other words, subjectivity isderivative of an existentially priorresponsibility to and for the other. Hisconception of ethical responsibility is thusalso a radical departure from the prevailingview of what it means to be a responsible moralagent. In this paper, I use jazz improvisationas a metaphor to focus on three interrelatedaspects of ethical responsibility on Levinas'saccount: passivity, heteronomy, andinescapability. I then point toward some waysin which reframing responsibility andsubjectivity along this line might offer newpossibilities for conceiving subjectivity andmoral agency in education. 相似文献
988.
John W. Toumbourou Ian Williams Primrose Letcher Ann Sanson Diana Smart 《Australian journal of psychology》2011,63(4):214-223
Using a community‐based Australian birth cohort, groups with distinct longitudinal profiles (trajectories) of internalising behaviour from early childhood through mid‐adolescence were identified for boys and girls from parent ratings. Six internalising trajectories were identified for both genders, comprising stable, decreasing, and increasing pathways. Hierarchical regression models predicted the contribution of internalising trajectories to self‐reported age 17 depressive symptoms for males (n = 557) and females (n = 633), after controlling for the effects of a range of adolescent precursors. Internalising trajectories and adolescent psychosocial factors, each contributed to the prediction of subsequent depressive symptoms, with girls and boys on increasing internalising trajectories and boys on a fluctuating high trajectory particularly at risk. Results suggest that parent ratings can identify coherent internalising behaviour pathways that originate early in life and influence subsequent adjustment. 相似文献
989.
990.