全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1886篇 |
免费 | 85篇 |
专业分类
1971篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 13篇 |
2020年 | 29篇 |
2019年 | 37篇 |
2018年 | 31篇 |
2017年 | 42篇 |
2016年 | 67篇 |
2015年 | 41篇 |
2014年 | 40篇 |
2013年 | 244篇 |
2012年 | 78篇 |
2011年 | 81篇 |
2010年 | 50篇 |
2009年 | 54篇 |
2008年 | 54篇 |
2007年 | 86篇 |
2006年 | 68篇 |
2005年 | 69篇 |
2004年 | 69篇 |
2003年 | 65篇 |
2002年 | 68篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 31篇 |
1999年 | 32篇 |
1998年 | 27篇 |
1997年 | 28篇 |
1996年 | 31篇 |
1995年 | 41篇 |
1994年 | 31篇 |
1993年 | 24篇 |
1992年 | 23篇 |
1991年 | 17篇 |
1990年 | 22篇 |
1989年 | 16篇 |
1988年 | 12篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 18篇 |
1983年 | 27篇 |
1982年 | 32篇 |
1981年 | 17篇 |
1980年 | 21篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 19篇 |
1977年 | 16篇 |
1976年 | 12篇 |
1975年 | 15篇 |
1974年 | 17篇 |
1973年 | 10篇 |
1968年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有1971条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
12.
Ann R. Eisenberg 《Argumentation》1987,1(2):113-125
The infant's first natural response when faced with opposition or when he opposes others' actions is to cry. As this kind of behavior becomes ineffective, the responses of the individuals with which he interacts force him to adopt more conventional — especially verbal — patterns of arguing, leading him to rational argumentation. The purpose of the present paper is to observe progressions in children's earliest verbal arguments and to see how and when they learn to adjust their strategies for different kinds of opponents (peers; parents; other adults). In order to examine the emergence of such persuasive strategies, and their distribution according to the different categories of opponents, systematic audiotape recordings of two Spanish-speaking girls between 2 and 3 years were analyzed. The data suggest that, although there were some differences in how the girls argued with parents versus peers, they were only beginning to adjust their speech to make it appropriate for one or the other type of listener. In general, they resorted to one dominant strategy for all listeners (insist, repeat, cry, scream, ...). Each girl, however, developed a small number of less frequently used strategies that she reserved for a subcategory of opponents. For example, one of them, Nancy, only threatened and insulted peers, while the other, Marisa, only used please and a temporizing strategy with parents. The girls used less adaptive, more agressive strategies (e.g. threats and insults) with their peers. With one exception, all of the girls' moves were self-centered. In fact, the girls had not yet reached the stage of rational argumentation. 相似文献
13.
Glenn M. McEvoy Richard W. Beatty H. John Bernardin 《Journal of business and psychology》1987,2(2):97-111
This study compared the predictive validity of two types of overall assessment center ratings (clinical and mechanical) and two types of performance ratings (supervisor and subordinate). Data were gathered on approximately 50 law enforcement agency managers over a four-year time span. Results indicated that clinical predictions were not significantly better than mechanical ones and that prior performance ratings were better predictors of future performance ratings than were assessment centers. Further, dimension ratings within the assessment center were found to be unrelated to ratings of the same dimensions on the job four years later, supporting the notion of subtle criterion contamination (Klimoski & Strickland, 1977) and raising questions about the central assumptions underlying the assessment center approach. 相似文献
14.
The effects of computerized office and factory automation are examined. An open systems framework is used to organize this literature. The review suggests that the benefits of technology are derived from theintermediate effects of the technology on organizational processes (the task structure, personnel system, formal structure, and informal organization). Thus, it is misleading to examine thedirect effects of computerized technology on organizational outcomes such as profits and satisfaction. Some of the effects of technology on the organizational processes are inevitable (e.g., changes in informal communication patterns). Others are determined less by the technology than by management decisions. The key to achieving success with computerized technology is matching changes in organizational processes to each other, as well as to the technology and the larger environment of the organization.Ann Majchrzak is currently Associate Professor of Human Factors at the Institute of Safety and Systems Management at the University of Southern California. She has recently written two books on the subject of technological change. Katherine J. Klein is an Assistant Professor of Industrial and Organizational Psychology in the Psychology Department of the University of Maryland at College Park. 相似文献
15.
The effect of self-control and compliance on the attentional performance of hyperactive children was assessed. Visual and auditory attention tasks were presented in conditions in which the experimenter was either absent and therefore not imposing external control, or present and therefore imposing some degree of control. There was no difference in performance between hyperactives and controls when the experimenter was present, but the hyperactives' performance showed a greater deterioration than controls when the experimenter was absent. The amount of movement displayed during the tasks was greater for hyperactives and increased more for this group during experimenter-absent conditions. The results support the contention that noncompliance is a major contributor to the poor performance of hyperactive children, which can be seen as an application deficit rather than an ability deficit. These findings have relevance for the current debate on the association between hyperactivity and conduct disorder, and from an applied perspective they serve to stress the importance of situational contributors to the problem behaviors of hyperactive children. 相似文献
16.
Ann R. Beck 《Brain and language》1984,22(2):320-338
The ability of aphasic subjects to process the sounds and meanings of ongoing speech was tested. Subjects, 10 aphasic and 20 control, heard test sentences which contained one member of phonemically similar word pairs in one of three semantic contexts: congruent, neutral, and noncongruent. Immediately after hearing a sentence, subjects were to indicate which member of the word pair had been in the sentence. All subject groups had similar overall patterns of response to the different semantic contexts. The division of aphasic subjects into groups of high and low comprehenders revealed response differences. High comprehending aphasic subjects, like control subjects, demonstrated interactive processing of the sounds and meaning of speech while low comprehending aphasic subjects did not. 相似文献
17.
Ann Jacob 《The Ecumenical review》2023,75(1):82-83
This is the text of the youth testimony at the opening prayer of the 11th Assembly of the World Council of Churches on 31 August 2022. 相似文献
18.
Ann T. Skinner Carmen Kealy Mette Ranta Aprile D. Benner Ersilia Menesini Ingrid Schoon 《Social and Personality Psychology Compass》2023,17(10):e12821
COVID-19 has altered adolescents' opportunities for developing and strengthening interpersonal skills and proficiencies. Using data from adolescents in Italy, the United States, and the United Kingdom, we examined the relation between internalizing symptoms assessed pre-pandemic or when pandemic-related restrictions were lifted (Time 1) and associated internalizing symptoms during a subsequent restrictive pandemic period (Time 2). Across all 3 countries, we found significant and consistent effect sizes in the relation between Time 1 and Time 2 internalizing symptoms. We further examined the direct and moderating impact of self-efficacy and contextual supports for adolescents' internalizing symptoms. Higher self-efficacy was associated with lower levels of internalizing symptoms at Time 2 in all 3 countries. Additionally, the relation between Time 1 and 2 internalizing symptoms was buffered by regulatory self-efficacy and peer support in Italy, but in the U.S., higher levels of general self-efficacy instead had an exacerbating effect on the relation between Times 1 and 2 internalizing symptoms. Results are discussed in the context of utilizing cross-national datasets to examine similarities in adolescent well-being over time and in the face of varying government responses to the pandemic. 相似文献
19.
A study was designed to compare several variables of Manpower Development Training Act (MDTA) clients who dropped out of the program with those of clients who completed the program. A random sample of application cards was drawn for individuals enrolled in MDTA adult classes in Milwaukee, Wisconsin. Trainees who completed the program were more likely to be married, older, characterized by a more stable employment history prior to entering the program, and slightly more educated than were program dropouts. Clients who completed the program versus those who did not were not significantly different on the following variables: (1) sex; (2) counseling versus no counseling; (3) race; and (4) client reported physical disability. 相似文献
20.