全文获取类型
收费全文 | 23468篇 |
免费 | 929篇 |
国内免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
24408篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 220篇 |
2019年 | 289篇 |
2018年 | 397篇 |
2017年 | 429篇 |
2016年 | 474篇 |
2015年 | 355篇 |
2014年 | 385篇 |
2013年 | 1960篇 |
2012年 | 695篇 |
2011年 | 717篇 |
2010年 | 433篇 |
2009年 | 412篇 |
2008年 | 540篇 |
2007年 | 625篇 |
2006年 | 527篇 |
2005年 | 506篇 |
2004年 | 479篇 |
2003年 | 469篇 |
2002年 | 499篇 |
2001年 | 765篇 |
2000年 | 738篇 |
1999年 | 566篇 |
1998年 | 237篇 |
1996年 | 246篇 |
1995年 | 236篇 |
1992年 | 470篇 |
1991年 | 457篇 |
1990年 | 447篇 |
1989年 | 414篇 |
1988年 | 428篇 |
1987年 | 389篇 |
1986年 | 400篇 |
1985年 | 396篇 |
1984年 | 321篇 |
1983年 | 294篇 |
1982年 | 232篇 |
1981年 | 219篇 |
1979年 | 361篇 |
1978年 | 268篇 |
1977年 | 225篇 |
1975年 | 292篇 |
1974年 | 368篇 |
1973年 | 373篇 |
1972年 | 293篇 |
1971年 | 285篇 |
1970年 | 283篇 |
1969年 | 251篇 |
1968年 | 354篇 |
1967年 | 308篇 |
1966年 | 284篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
53.
54.
55.
56.
Ratings of pre-morbid intelligence level, derived from the hospital charts of 26 chronic schizophrenics, were correlated with the post-mortem brain volume measures of cortex, white matter, central grey matter, and ventricles. Contrary to hypothesis, no significant correlations were found. 相似文献
57.
58.
Fighting pairs of isolated DBA/2 mice showed a significant increase in tail-flick response latencies independent of whether opponents were losing or winning the combat. The effect lasted less than 10 min in both animals. Elevated pain thresholds were also found in isolates that attacked a nonaggressive conspecific, and were prevented by naltrexone (0.2 mg/kg), while a larger dose (1.0 mg/kg) inhibited the attack behavior. A small increase in pain threshold was observed after exposure of isolates to the test box alone, while isolation per se had no effect on baseline tail-flick latencies. The data demonstrate that endogenous pain suppressing systems are activated during attack and suggest that this opioid-mediated antinociception is a correlate of the isolation syndrome, reflecting enhanced arousal of the attacking animal. 相似文献
59.
The influence of affect on causal attributions for success and failure was examined in this experiment. A positive, neurtral, or negative mood was induced in subjects who then learned they had either succeeded or failed an aptitude test taken previously. Relative to neutral mood control conditions, subjects in both positive and negative mood conditions showed a pronounced self-serving bias, particularly following success. The finding is interpreted as self-regulation of affective state. Specifically, causal attribution of success to internal factors can sustain or enhance positive affect; attribution of failure to external factors can diminish negative affect. Ancillary analyses corroborated this interpretation. 相似文献
60.
In the mind of many people chance and luck act as real but different causes of events. Even in strictly defined situations as casino gambling, people may perceive influences of luck that help to overcome the negative expectancy defined by the rules of chance. Interviews with gamblers in casinos confirmed this idea. In two experiments it was established that the distinction between chance and luck are also made by ordinary subjects in everyday situations. The results revealed that chance is perceived to operate when an event is surprising, an unexpected coincidence. Luck is perceived when an event implies the escape from negative consequences, or the achievement of something that is important and difficult. The distinction between chance and luck can explain why people are trapped by the illusion of control, even when it is clear that they have no influence on the physical causation determining the outcomes of events. They cannot change the outcome of the roulette wheel, but they can employ their luck, which helps them to place their bets on the winning number. 相似文献