全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1823篇 |
免费 | 34篇 |
专业分类
1857篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 12篇 |
2023年 | 19篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 13篇 |
2020年 | 40篇 |
2019年 | 51篇 |
2018年 | 67篇 |
2017年 | 70篇 |
2016年 | 70篇 |
2015年 | 47篇 |
2014年 | 61篇 |
2013年 | 194篇 |
2012年 | 115篇 |
2011年 | 119篇 |
2010年 | 54篇 |
2009年 | 52篇 |
2008年 | 77篇 |
2007年 | 87篇 |
2006年 | 75篇 |
2005年 | 53篇 |
2004年 | 64篇 |
2003年 | 63篇 |
2002年 | 52篇 |
2001年 | 34篇 |
2000年 | 16篇 |
1999年 | 35篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 17篇 |
1995年 | 14篇 |
1994年 | 14篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 18篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 10篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 13篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
1969年 | 5篇 |
1966年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有1857条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
141.
A new grouping principle, uniform connectedness (UC), has been posited to be a basic organizer of visual pattern information,
one that takes precedence over other, more classic grouping principles (Palmer & Rock, 1994), but its ontogenetic origins
have not previously been investigated. We examined whether 3- to 4-month-olds and 6- to 7-month-olds utilize UC to organize
static two-dimensional displays. Infants habituated to uniformly connected patterns exhibited a novelty preference for disconnected
element patterns, whereas those without any habituation failed to exhibit a preference. The results indicate that infants
are sensitive to UC as a cue for perceptual organization. Prior studies indicate that some Gestalt principles (e.g., common
movement) are functional during the first half year of life, but that other principles (e.g., form similarity) are less readily
available. The present finding showing that young infants are sensitive to UC points to the foundational nature of this cue
and adds to an emerging body of evidence indicating that at least some of the mechanisms believed to produce perceptual organization
in adults are already operational in the first months of life. nt]mis|This research was supported by grants from the National
Science Foundation (BCS 0224240) and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (HD042451). 相似文献
142.
Eliot R. Smith Daniel A. Miller Angela T. Maitner Teresa Garcia-Marques 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2006,42(4):471-478
Two experiments show that repeated exposure to information about a target person reduces individuation and thereby increases stereotyping of the target person based on social group memberships. The effect is not due to familiarity-induced liking (the mere exposure effect), nor is it mediated by increased accessibility of the target’s social category, nor by increases in perceived social judgeability. The results are most consistent with the use of feelings of familiarity as a regulator of processing mode, such that familiar objects receive less systematic or analytic processing. In everyday life, frequent exposure to another person ordinarily produces not only familiarity but also liking, individuated knowledge, and friendship, factors that may effectively limit stereotyping. But when previous exposure is unconfounded from these other factors, its effect can be to increase stereotyping. 相似文献
143.
Dion, Berscheid, and Walster (1972), in their seminal article, labeled the attribution of positive characteristics to attractive people the "beauty-is-good" stereotype. The stereotyping literature since then provides extensive evidence for the differential judgment and treatment of attractive versus unattractive people, but does not indicate whether it is an advantage to be attractive or a disadvantage to be unattractive. Two studies investigated the direction of attractiveness stereotyping by comparing judgments of positive and negative attributes for medium vs. low and medium vs. high attractive faces. Taken together, results for adults (Experiment 1) and children (Experiment 2) suggest that most often, unattractiveness is a disadvantage, consistent with negativity bias (e.g., Rozin & Royzman, 2001) but contrary to the "beauty-is-good" aphorism. 相似文献
144.
This study examines the antecedents and outcomes of perceived similarity in mentoring relationships in a sample of 82 matched mentor-protégé dyads. Polynomial regression analyses were used to examine the association between actual and perceived similarity. Protégés were more likely to see themselves as similar to their mentors when they and their mentors shared either higher or lower levels of attachment security. However, sharing similar levels of relational self-construal was unrelated to protégés perceptions of similarity with their mentors. Path analyses revealed that protégés reported more organizational and professional commitment when they saw themselves as similar to their mentors, and that these associations were mediated by protégés’ reports of their mentors providing role modeling functions. These findings suggest that the effects of actual mentor-protégé similarity on perceptions of similarity may depend on the shared attribute, and provide further evidence of the importance of perceived similarity to not only mentoring relationships, but also to organizations. 相似文献
145.
146.
147.
Behavioral contrast in fixed-interval components: effects of extinction-component duration.
下载免费PDF全文

J C de Rose 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1986,45(2):175-188
Seven albino rats were exposed to a multiple schedule of reinforcement in which the two components (fixed interval and extinction) alternated such that a presentation of the extinction component followed each fixed-interval reinforcement. In baseline sessions, the duration of the extinction component was constant and always one-third of the fixed-interval value. Probe sessions contained a probe segment in which the duration of the extinction component was increased; the response rate in fixed-interval components during the probe segment was compared with the response rate in the segments preceding and following the probe. The effect of increasing the duration of the extinction component was studied under three values of fixed interval: 30 s, 120 s, and 18 s, in three successive conditions. Response rate within fixed intervals was a direct function of duration of the extinction component. Pausing at the beginning of the fixed interval decreased as extinction duration increased. These effects were larger and more consistent for the shorter fixed-interval values (18 s and 30 s). These results indicate a functional relation between relative component duration and responding. For the component providing more frequent reinforcement, this could be stated as an inverse relationship between relative component duration and response rate. This relation is similar to findings regarding the ratio of trial and intertrial duration in Pavlovian conditioning procedures, and suggests that behavioral contrast may be related to Pavlovian contingencies underlying the multiple schedule. 相似文献
148.
Lynn Hasher Rose T. Zacks Ellen R. Stoltzfus Michael J. Kane S. Lisa Connelly 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》1996,3(2):231-237
In two experiments, the pattern of persistence of negative priming effects across delay intervals of 500 and 2,500 msec was assessed using a within-subjects, random sequencing of delays. Neill and Valdes (1992; Neill, Valdes, Terry, & Gorfein, 1992) have argued that a within-subject experimental design is required for decay of negative priming to be seen, in contrast to results reported elsewhere (e.g., Tipper, Weaver, Cameron, Brehaut, & Bastedo, 1991) showing stable negative priming effects across delays. In neither experiment was substantial evidence of decay detected, raising questions for the notion that suppression necessarily declines across brief temporal intervals and for the assertion that episodic retrieval is the sole source of negative priming. 相似文献
149.
The Impact of Procedural Justice on Opinions of Public Policy: Solid Waste Management as an Example1
This study assessed the applicability of Tyler and Lind's (1992) relational model of authority to the context of local government policies on solid waste management. Tests of the hypothesized relations proposed by Tyler and Lind, accomplished through structural equation modeling, revealed that our data supported the relations between the various constructs of the model. Perceptions of both process control and relational process were found to be positively related to procedural fairness judgments, while procedural fairness judgments were weakly related to evaluations of government entities. Contrary to our expectations, a similar structural equation model test of an extension of the Tyler and Lind model showed that the model constructs were unrelated to respondents' opinions of public policies. The results also indicated that respondents living in communities with different infrastructures and political climates differed slightly in their perceptions of procedural justice and related constructs, but did not differ in their endorsement of the various solid waste management policies. 相似文献
150.
Each subject performed two tasks, dividing a line segment so that either (a) theratio of subjective lengths corresponded to the ratio of the magnitudes of two numerals or (b) thedifference in length was proportional to the numerical difference. Had subjects actually performed two operations on the same scale, the responses would have been nonmonotonically related. Instead, data for the two tasks were nearly identical and ordinally compatible with either a ratio or a subtractive model. The ratio model implied scale values for numerals that were a positively accelerated function of numerical value, inconsistent with previous results. With a nonlinear response function for graphic length, the subtractive model fit well, yielding scale values that were a negatively accelerated function of numerical value and a linear function of previously obtained scales. These results, together with other recent findings, suggest that subjects may perform the same operation in spite of instructions to judge “ratios” or “differences” and that this operation can be best represented by a subtractive model. 相似文献