首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   32007篇
  免费   1384篇
  国内免费   25篇
  33416篇
  2020年   362篇
  2019年   458篇
  2018年   642篇
  2017年   611篇
  2016年   693篇
  2015年   469篇
  2014年   558篇
  2013年   2515篇
  2012年   1064篇
  2011年   1059篇
  2010年   618篇
  2009年   689篇
  2008年   877篇
  2007年   905篇
  2006年   797篇
  2005年   695篇
  2004年   685篇
  2003年   630篇
  2002年   644篇
  2001年   1060篇
  2000年   1059篇
  1999年   790篇
  1998年   330篇
  1997年   304篇
  1995年   318篇
  1992年   635篇
  1991年   585篇
  1990年   568篇
  1989年   545篇
  1988年   540篇
  1987年   483篇
  1986年   482篇
  1985年   530篇
  1984年   436篇
  1983年   402篇
  1982年   317篇
  1979年   471篇
  1978年   328篇
  1977年   301篇
  1976年   320篇
  1975年   363篇
  1974年   433篇
  1973年   462篇
  1972年   400篇
  1971年   342篇
  1970年   317篇
  1969年   334篇
  1968年   417篇
  1967年   370篇
  1966年   357篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
751.
752.
753.
754.
755.
756.
757.
Two doses diethylpropion, one dose pipradrol, one dose amobarbital and placebo were administered to 116 subjects, who rated their state on twenty variables. The variables were intercorrelated under pipradrol and pentymal. The correlation matrices were subjected to factor analysis. Four factors were found in both studies: Happiness, Alertness, Relaxation, and Flight of thoughts. There is some agreement between the two factor analyses. There were few significant effects of the drugs on factor scores, but the structure is quite unambiguous: pipradrol and diethylpropion are stimulating, and amobarbital tranquilizing.  相似文献   
758.
Response latencies in naming objects   总被引:16,自引:0,他引:16  
After some preliminary analysis of what is involved in naming objects, in which the possible role of classificatory systems in the memory store is discussed, it is shown experimentally that there are consistent differences between the times taken to respond to presented objects by uttering their names, variations between the performances of different individuals being outweighed by variations due to the different objects. Moreover, there is a high consistency between different individuals as to the ordering of objects in respect of their naming latencies. It is further shown that a high correlation exists between the time taken to name an object and the frequency with which its name occurs in the language as a whole, as estimated in the Thorndike-Lorge Word List. Some implications of these findings are discussed, especially with reference to possible mechanisms by which presented objects are visually identified, and the appropriate names retrieved from the “word-store.”  相似文献   
759.
Memory performance awareness in younger and older adults   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Reports on the course of memory-monitoring skills across adulthood are discrepant in conclusions and limited in scope. The purpose of this study was to build a large data base (3 samples and 7 different tasks) to assess performance awareness. Younger (19-41 years) and older (59-93 years) Ss estimated performance either before (i.e., predictions) or after (i.e., postdictions) completing each task. Predictions were less accurate than postdictions at both age levels, suggesting Ss monitored performance during the study-test cycle. Overall, the data suggested no consistent age effects in performance awareness: Age differences in monitoring occurred only in predictions and only for some tasks. Between-tasks differences in age effects could not be attributed to a single mediating mechanism like those suggested in previous reports. Why previous research has produced conflicting conclusions about metacognitive development in adulthood is discussed in light of these data.  相似文献   
760.
The usual superiority in frequency-of-occurrence judgments of younger vs. older subjects was hypothesized to result from greater strategic encoding of the materials conveying frequency information. A subject-paced, visual search task was designed to control nontarget word encoding. Relative frequency judgments for the nontarget word pairs were equally accurate for younger and older subjects, and performance of both groups was above chance. Results suggest that strategic cover-task encoding can induce age differences in incidental frequency processing. Consistent with a nonoptimal as opposed to an optimal view, automatic processes require only minimal capacity for above-chance performance, but additional strategic resources can increase performance. When such additional resources are used more by younger than by older subjects, the former are better in frequency performance.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号