全文获取类型
收费全文 | 999篇 |
免费 | 53篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
1054篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 13篇 |
2022年 | 15篇 |
2021年 | 25篇 |
2020年 | 31篇 |
2019年 | 19篇 |
2018年 | 43篇 |
2017年 | 50篇 |
2016年 | 42篇 |
2015年 | 25篇 |
2014年 | 26篇 |
2013年 | 153篇 |
2012年 | 69篇 |
2011年 | 80篇 |
2010年 | 47篇 |
2009年 | 30篇 |
2008年 | 52篇 |
2007年 | 44篇 |
2006年 | 37篇 |
2005年 | 34篇 |
2004年 | 28篇 |
2003年 | 34篇 |
2002年 | 17篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 13篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有1054条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
81.
Previous studies on sustained attention ability in the elderly produced inconsistent results. The aim of this study was to evaluate sustained attention performance in younger and older individuals by using, in a within-subjects design, two versions of the same task (the sustained attention to response task, SART) in which only in the response mode differed: in a traditionally formatted task (TFT), subjects had to respond to rare targets, and in a Go/No-Go task they had to withhold response to rare targets. Results showed that in the TFT SART only the older group exhibited a vigilance decrement. On the contrary, only young individuals showed a vigilance decrement in the Go/No-Go SART. These results showed that older individuals, who also reported less mind wandering and a higher level of motivation, exhibited preserved sustained attention ability in the Go/No-Go SART, which could be explained by increased engagement of cognitive control mechanisms in this population. The discrepancy in performance depending on the approach used also underlines the need for further studies on the nature of attention failures and their underlying mechanisms. 相似文献
82.
Ferrari V Didierjean A Marmèche E 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2006,59(2):397-410
The present study examines the dynamic aspects of perceptual processes in expert chess players. This topic is approached in terms of the anticipation processes carried out by experienced players during the encoding of chess positions. The aim of the first experiment, which used a short-term comparison task, was to stress the role of anticipation, which allows expert players to focus their attention on the area of the studied position where they expect the likely standard move to occur. The second experiment used a long-term recognition task. The results showed that expert players made many false recognitions on the new positions that could be expected from the positions presented in the preliminary study phase. Taken together, the results of the two experiments highlight the anticipatory component of expert perception. 相似文献
83.
Marie Carrière 《Journal of Academic Ethics》2006,4(1-4):245-260
A feminist perspective on selfhood – bound to a perspective on otherness – is the main concern of this article. The resonance of this notion of selfhood both with ethical philosophy and with the language of humanism enables a deeper understanding of a feminist ethics as well as its internal tensions. The article considers the relationship of feminism and humanism as one of “paradoxical fluidity” rather than antithetical polarization, to explore the ways in which feminism’s alliance with contemporary ethics exemplifies its paradoxical relation to humanism. The study then underlines the vital contribution of feminist discourse to an ethical understanding of selfhood and intersubjectivity. Finally, it examines the work of experimental Canadian poet Lola Lemire Tostevin, who reveals the importance of an ethical, feminist version of selfhood that highlights the insufficiency as well as the potential of both humanist and postmodern versions of subjectivity. 相似文献
84.
Lo?c Lemoine Kjerstin Torre Delignières Didier 《Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale》2006,60(4):247-257
A number of recent papers have suggested that the series of time intervals produced in continuation tapping may have fractal properties. This proposition, nevertheless, was only based on the visual appraisal of graphical results, and was not statistically supported. In the present study, we applied the ARMA/ARFIMA modeling procedures proposed by Wagenmakers, Farrell, and Ratcliff (2005) to test for the presence of long-range dependencies in continuation tapping data. Our results demonstrate the presence of long-range dependencies in most series and offer strong support for the hypothesis that fluctuations in tapping series are fractal in nature. 相似文献
85.
86.
Sebastian Sunday Grève 《Australasian journal of philosophy》2018,96(1):168-182
This essay discusses Wittgenstein's conception of logic, early and late, and some of the types of logical system that he constructed. The essay shows that the common view according to which Wittgenstein had stopped engaging in logic as a philosophical discipline by the time of writing Philosophical Investigations is mistaken. It is argued that, on the contrary, logic continued to figure at the very heart of later Wittgenstein's philosophy; and that Wittgenstein's mature philosophy of logic contains many interesting thoughts that have gone widely unnoticed. 相似文献
87.
Jo?lle Carpentier Geneviève A. Mageau Robert J. Vallerand 《Journal of Happiness Studies》2012,13(3):501-518
Research shows that harmonious and obsessive passion are positively and negatively linked to well-being respectively (e.g., Vallerand et al. in J Personal 75:505?C534, 2007; Psychol Sport Exerc 9:373?C392, 2008). The present study investigated the psychological mechanisms underlying the different impact of the two types of passion on well-being. A theoretical model involving passion, ruminations, flow experiences, and well-being was tested. Results showed that the more people have a harmonious passion, the more they tend to experience flow in their favorite activity, which in turn predicts higher well-being. Obsessive passion did not seem to be systematically linked to flow in the favorite activity. In contrast, the more people have an obsessive passion, the more they tend to ruminate about their passionate activity while engaging in another activity, which did not seem to be systematically the case for people with a harmonious passion. These ruminations are negatively related to flow experiences in the other activity, which are positively associated with well-being. Flow and ruminations thus seem to contribute to the understanding of the link between passion and well-being. 相似文献
88.
Non-human primates possess species-specific repertoires of acoustically distinct call types that can be found in adults in
predictable ways. Evidence for vocal flexibility is generally rare and typically restricted to acoustic variants within the
main call types or sequential production of multiple calls. So far, evidence for context-specific call sequences has been
mainly in relation to external disturbances, particularly predation. In this study, we investigated extensively the vocal
behaviour of free-ranging and individually identified Diana monkeys in non-predatory contexts. We found that adult females
produced four vocal structures alone (‘H’, ‘L’, ‘R’ and ‘A’ calls, the latter consisting of two subtypes) or combined in non-random
ways (‘HA’, ‘LA’ and ‘RA’ call combinations) in relation to ongoing behaviour or external events. Specifically, the concatenation
of an introductory call with the most frequently emitted and contextually neutral ‘A’ call seems to function as a contextual
refiner of this potential individual identifier. Our results demonstrate that some non-human primates are able to increase
the effective size of their small vocal repertoire not only by varying the acoustic structure of basic call types but also
by combining them into more complex structures. We have demonstrated this phenomenon for a category of vocalisations with
a purely social function and discuss the implications of these findings for evolutionary theories of primate vocal communication. 相似文献
89.
Mathieu B. Brodeur Eva Kehayia Geneviève Dion-Lessard Mélissa Chauret Tina Montreuil Emmanuelle Dionne-Dostie Martin Lepage 《Behavior research methods》2012,44(4):961-970
Throughout the last decades, numerous picture data sets have been developed, such as the Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980) set, and have been normalized for variables such as name and familiarity; however, due to cultural and linguistic differences, norms can vary from one country to another. The effect due specifically to culture has already been demonstrated by comparing samples from different countries where the same language is spoken. On the other hand, it is still not clear how differences between languages may affect norms. The present study explores this issue by collecting and comparing norms on names and many other features from French Canadian speakers and English Canadian speakers living in Montreal, who thus live in similar cultural environments. Norms were collected for the photos of objects from the Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS) by asking participants to name the objects, to categorize them, and to rate their familiarity, visual complexity, object agreement, viewpoint agreement, and manipulability. Names and ratings from the French speakers are available in Appendix A, available in the supplemental materials. The results show that most of the norms are comparable across linguistic groups and also that the ratings given are correlated across linguistic groups. The only significant group differences were found in viewpoint agreement and visual complexity. Overall, there was good concordance between the norms collected from French and English native speakers living in the same cultural setting. 相似文献
90.
Tania Lecomte Marie-Christine Laferrière-Simard Claude Leclerc 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》2012,42(2):55-61
The therapeutic alliance is considered an essential component of any clinical or psychological intervention. The therapeutic
alliance can however change over time, and therefore it is relevant to determine if the alliance and its variations are related
to clinical outcomes. The present study had three objectives: (1) determine the impact of the alliance measured by the client
and the therapist, on clinical and psychosocial outcomes of individuals with early psychosis receiving group interventions;
(2) measure the concordance between the client and the therapists’ ratings of the alliance; and (3) determine the best predictor
of the client’s alliance in our sample. A total of 36 individuals with early psychosis who participated in group interventions
for symptom management answered the WAI three times, and so did the group therapists. Outcomes included measures of insight,
symptoms (total, positive and negative), and self-esteem. Attendance and group participation were also measured. Results suggest
that the overall client’s alliance predicted total symptoms and self-esteem at post-therapy, whereas both clients’ and therapists’
ratings predicted group attendance and participation. Greater variations in the alliance overtime for the clients were somewhat
linked to worse negative symptoms at post-therapy. Alliance ratings were generally high and fairly concordant between therapists
and clients. Only the baseline measure of capacity for attachment, predicted the client’s overall alliance scores. Clinical
implications are discussed. 相似文献