全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4610篇 |
免费 | 78篇 |
专业分类
4688篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 28篇 |
2023年 | 47篇 |
2022年 | 40篇 |
2021年 | 61篇 |
2020年 | 116篇 |
2019年 | 142篇 |
2018年 | 163篇 |
2017年 | 181篇 |
2016年 | 180篇 |
2015年 | 128篇 |
2014年 | 162篇 |
2013年 | 552篇 |
2012年 | 224篇 |
2011年 | 259篇 |
2010年 | 144篇 |
2009年 | 175篇 |
2008年 | 215篇 |
2007年 | 162篇 |
2006年 | 173篇 |
2005年 | 152篇 |
2004年 | 168篇 |
2003年 | 149篇 |
2002年 | 122篇 |
2001年 | 52篇 |
2000年 | 54篇 |
1999年 | 36篇 |
1998年 | 47篇 |
1997年 | 50篇 |
1996年 | 41篇 |
1995年 | 33篇 |
1994年 | 25篇 |
1993年 | 22篇 |
1992年 | 33篇 |
1991年 | 30篇 |
1990年 | 23篇 |
1989年 | 22篇 |
1988年 | 26篇 |
1987年 | 33篇 |
1986年 | 34篇 |
1985年 | 28篇 |
1984年 | 35篇 |
1983年 | 39篇 |
1982年 | 28篇 |
1981年 | 22篇 |
1980年 | 35篇 |
1979年 | 27篇 |
1978年 | 17篇 |
1977年 | 23篇 |
1976年 | 23篇 |
1974年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有4688条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
901.
902.
We analyze four general signal detection models for recognition memory that differ in their distributional assumptions. Our
analyses show that a basic assumption of signal detection theory, the likelihood ratio decision axis, implies three regularities
in recognition memory: (1) the mirror effect, (2) the variance effect, and (3) the z-ROC length effect. For each model, we
present the equations that produce the three regularities and show, in computed examples, how they do so. We then show that
the regularities appear in data from a range of recognition studies. The analyses and data in our study support the following
generalization: Individuals make efficient recognition decisions on the basis of likelihood ratios. 相似文献
903.
Vincent Chong Khatharya Um Monica Hahn David Pheng Clifford Yee Colette Auerswald 《Aggression and violent behavior》2009,14(6):461-469
There is a nascent, but growing, literature on Southeast Asian youth violence that focuses on the role of acculturation, portraying violence as a problem of maladaptation. However, scholars overlook the ways in which violence holds meaning for the youth who experience it and how violence may be related to racial and gender identity formation. We conducted a qualitative study with young Southeast Asian men to elicit the role violence plays in their understanding of what it means for them to be Southeast Asian and male. We conducted focus groups and semi-structured individual interviews with an ethnically diverse group of 21 young Southeast Asian men 13–17 years of age from Alameda and Contra Costa Counties, California. Data were analyzed using the extended case method approach. Our findings illustrate that violence and engagement with community-based organizations are situational tools that these young Southeast Asian men use to navigate their social contexts in an attempt to be resilient in ecological contexts marked by alienation and discrimination, as well as to construct accepted and successful racial and gender identities. Furthermore, we found that their actions were guided by gendered codes of conduct, such as a “code of the street.” 相似文献
904.
Andrew Sayer 《Res Publica》2009,15(1):1-16
The dominant focus of thinking about economic justice is overwhelmingly distributive, that is, concerned with what people
get in terms of resources and opportunities. It views work mainly negatively, as a burden or cost, or else is neutral about it,
rather than seeing it as a source of meaning and fulfilment—a good in its own right. However, what we do in life has at least as much, if not more, influence on whom we become, as does what we get. Thus we have good reason also to be concerned with what Paul Gomberg has termed contributive
justice, that is, justice as regards what people are expected and able to contribute in terms of work. Complex, interesting work
allows workers not only to develop and exercise their capacities, and gain the satisfaction from achieving the internal goods
of a practice, but to gain the external goods of recognition and esteem. As Gomberg’s analysis of the concept of contributive
justice in relation to equality of opportunity shows, as long as the more satisfying kinds of work are concentrated into a
subset of jobs, rather than shared out among all jobs, then many workers will be denied the chance to have meaningful work
and the recognition that goes with it. In this paper I examine the contributive justice argument, suggest how it can be further
strengthened, arguing, inter alia, that ignoring contributive injustice tends to support legitimations of distributive inequality.
相似文献
Andrew SayerEmail: |
905.
The question why synaesthesia, an atypical binding within or between modalities, occurs is both enduring and important. Two explanations have been provided: (1) a congenital explanation: we are all born as synaesthetes but most of us subsequently lose the experience due to brain development; (2) a learning explanation: synaesthesia is related to some learning process during childhood. Three recent studies provide conflicting support for these explanations. Two studies supported the idea that synaesthesia is learned by showing that the frequency of everyday language implicitly modulates the synaesthetic experience. Another study argued that synaesthesia reflects basic, innate magnitude representations. In this paper we reassess these points of view, and show that it is possible for both to be valid. These findings are integrated into an interactive specialization account of development in order to explain the neuronal mechanism underlying synaesthesia. 相似文献
906.
907.
908.
909.
Ellis, Ansorge and Lavidor (2007) [Ellis, A.W., Ansorge, L., & Lavidor, M. (2007). Words, hemispheres, and dissociable subsystems: The effects of exposure duration, case alternation, priming and continuity of form on word recognition in the left and right visual fields. Brain and Language, 103, 292–303.] presented three experiments investigating the effects of exposure duration and format (normal vs mixed or alternating case) on the recognition of words presented in the left and right visual fields. The experiments were conducted within the framework of ‘dissociable neural subsystems theory’ developed by Marsolek and colleagues. Marsolek and Deason (2007) [Marsolek, C.J. & Deason, R.G. (2007). Hemispheric asymmetries in visual word-form processing: Progress, conflict, and evaluating theories. Brain and Language, 103, 304–307.] offer some reflections on our paper, including alternative interpretations of some of our findings. We respond to those reflections in this reply. 相似文献
910.
Ghorashi SM Smilek D Di Lollo V 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2007,33(1):124-136
J. S. Joseph, M. M. Chun, and K. Nakayama (1997) found that pop-out visual search was impaired as a function of intertarget lag in an attentional blink (AB) paradigm in which the 1st target was a letter and the 2nd target was a search display. In 4 experiments, the present authors tested the implication that search efficiency should be similarly impaired (steeper search slopes at shorter lags). A conventional AB deficit was found, but, contrary to expectations, search slopes were invariant with lag. These results suggest that no search can be carried out during the period of the AB. Instead, the search is postponed until after the 1st target has been processed. The authors conclude that efficient visual search cannot be carried out unless the visual system is configured appropriately for the search task. If the initial configuration is inappropriate, processing of the 2nd target is held in abeyance until the system has been suitably reconfigured. 相似文献