全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4003篇 |
免费 | 69篇 |
专业分类
4072篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 24篇 |
2023年 | 43篇 |
2022年 | 34篇 |
2021年 | 48篇 |
2020年 | 98篇 |
2019年 | 132篇 |
2018年 | 142篇 |
2017年 | 165篇 |
2016年 | 157篇 |
2015年 | 110篇 |
2014年 | 141篇 |
2013年 | 495篇 |
2012年 | 189篇 |
2011年 | 223篇 |
2010年 | 129篇 |
2009年 | 148篇 |
2008年 | 193篇 |
2007年 | 136篇 |
2006年 | 147篇 |
2005年 | 134篇 |
2004年 | 144篇 |
2003年 | 128篇 |
2002年 | 110篇 |
2001年 | 44篇 |
2000年 | 51篇 |
1999年 | 30篇 |
1998年 | 42篇 |
1997年 | 43篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 25篇 |
1994年 | 22篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 31篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 25篇 |
1989年 | 27篇 |
1988年 | 21篇 |
1987年 | 29篇 |
1986年 | 29篇 |
1985年 | 25篇 |
1984年 | 31篇 |
1983年 | 33篇 |
1982年 | 23篇 |
1981年 | 16篇 |
1980年 | 29篇 |
1979年 | 21篇 |
1978年 | 17篇 |
1977年 | 21篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 10篇 |
排序方式: 共有4072条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
911.
912.
913.
Sarah McMahon N. Andrew Peterson Samantha C. Winter Jane E. Palmer Judy L. Postmus Ruth Anne Koenick 《American journal of community psychology》2015,56(1-2):46-56
Bystander intervention has been increasingly applied to prevent sexual violence on college campuses. Its underlying theory assumes unidirectional relationships between variables, predicting that bystander behaviors (i.e., actions taken to intervene in sexual violence situations) will be influenced by bystander intentions (BI; i.e., likelihood to intervene in the future), which in turn will be affected by bystander efficacy (BE; i.e., confidence to intervene). One question for theory is whether a reciprocal relationship exists between BI and BE. We used structural equation modeling (SEM) with longitudinal data to test unidirectional and reciprocal causal relations between BI and BE. Participants (n = 1390) were students at a northeastern US university. Four models were examined using SEM: (1) a baseline model with autoregressive paths; (2) a model with autoregressive effects and BI predicting future BE; (3) a model with autoregressive effects and BE predicting future BI; and, (4) a fully cross‐lagged model. Results indicated that reciprocal causality was found to occur between BI and BE. In addition, a final model demonstrated indirect effects of a bystander intervention program on bystander behaviors through both BI and BE at different time points. Implications for theory and practice are described, and directions for future research discussed. 相似文献
914.
915.
Nualnong Wongtongkam Paul R. Ward Andrew Day Anthony H. Winefield 《Deviant behavior》2015,36(3):187-199
Physical violence in Bangkok schools has been the subject of considerable public concern in recent years and yet relatively little is known about the nature of violence perpetrated by vocational college students. This study sought to understand the reasons for such violence through a series of semi-structured interviews with 32 male students. The analysis identified revenge from previous fights as a key motivation for violence. Students described a range of different responses to threats of violence, including renting safe accommodation and concealing weapons. These findings are discussed in relation to how an understanding of cross-cultural risk factors for violence is important for the development of effective prevention strategies. 相似文献
916.
We report on five experiments investigating response choices and response times to simple science questions that evoke student “misconceptions,” and we construct a simple model to explain the patterns of response choices. Physics students were asked to compare a physical quantity represented by the slope, such as speed, on simple physics graphs. We found that response times of incorrect answers, resulting from comparing heights, were faster than response times of correct answers comparing slopes. This result alone might be explained by the fact that height was typically processed faster than slope for this kind of task, which we confirmed in a separate experiment. However, we hypothesize that the difference in response time is an indicator of the cause (rather than the result) of the response choice. To support this, we found that imposing a 3‐s delay in responding increased the number of students comparing slopes (answering correctly) on the task. Additionally a significant proportion of students recognized the correct written rule (compare slope), but on the graph task they incorrectly compared heights. Finally, training either with repetitive examples or providing a general rule both improved scores, but only repetitive examples had a large effect on response times, thus providing evidence of dual paths or processes to a solution. Considering models of heuristics, information accumulation models, and models relevant to the Stroop effect, we construct a simple relative processing time model that could be viewed as a kind of fluency heuristic. The results suggest that misconception‐like patterns of answers to some science questions commonly found on tests may be explained in part by automatic processes that involve the relative processing time of considered dimensions and a priority to answer quickly. 相似文献
917.
Continuous white noise played through headphones was used as a classroom intervention to reduce off-task behavior and increase assignment production and accuracy for a child with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). When listening to white noise the student's off-task behavior decreased from 89% during baseline to 62%. Subsequently, he began taking a stimulant medication for his ADHD. Pharmacotherapy alone (65% off task) was approximately equivalent to white noise alone. When medication was combined with white noise his off-task behavior decreased to 45%. Assignment production was also enhanced while listening to white noise, whereas accuracy remained high throughout. 相似文献
918.
919.
Andrew Singleton 《Journal of Beliefs & Values》2015,36(2):239-243
The number of people with no religious affiliation (religious ‘nones’) is growing in the west. However, little is known about the religiosity – or non-religiosity – of these people. Using representative survey data, this report examines the levels of religious belief and practice among Australian religious nones and compares this with those who identify with a religion. It is found that there is a deep congruence with no religious affiliation and other ways of being considered non-religious. 相似文献
920.
Andrew D. Case Nathan R. Todd Michael J. Kral 《American journal of community psychology》2014,54(1-2):60-71
Community psychology recognizes the need for research methods that illuminate context, culture, diversity, and process. One such method, ethnography, has crossed into multiple disciplines from anthropology, and indeed, community psychologists are becoming community ethnographers. Ethnographic work stands at the intersection of bridging universal questions with the particularities of people and groups bounded in time, geographic location, and social location. Ethnography is thus historical and deeply contextual, enabling a rich, in-depth understanding of communities that is aligned with the values and goals of community psychology. The purpose of this paper is to elucidate the potential of ethnography for community psychology and to encourage its use within the field as a method to capture culture and context, to document process, and to reveal how social change and action occur within and through communities. We discuss the method of ethnography, draw connections to community psychology values and goals, and identify tensions from our experiences doing ethnography. Overall, we assert that ethnography is a method that resonates with community psychology and present this paper as a resource for those interested in using this method in their research or community activism. 相似文献