首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4798篇
  免费   290篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2023年   47篇
  2022年   36篇
  2021年   54篇
  2020年   114篇
  2019年   149篇
  2018年   174篇
  2017年   187篇
  2016年   175篇
  2015年   124篇
  2014年   162篇
  2013年   602篇
  2012年   220篇
  2011年   263篇
  2010年   140篇
  2009年   166篇
  2008年   236篇
  2007年   179篇
  2006年   184篇
  2005年   171篇
  2004年   170篇
  2003年   154篇
  2002年   134篇
  2001年   69篇
  2000年   70篇
  1999年   56篇
  1998年   63篇
  1997年   48篇
  1996年   53篇
  1995年   39篇
  1994年   33篇
  1993年   25篇
  1992年   38篇
  1991年   30篇
  1990年   31篇
  1989年   34篇
  1988年   36篇
  1987年   36篇
  1986年   39篇
  1985年   40篇
  1984年   45篇
  1983年   41篇
  1982年   37篇
  1981年   27篇
  1980年   38篇
  1979年   38篇
  1978年   24篇
  1977年   34篇
  1976年   30篇
  1974年   22篇
  1973年   26篇
排序方式: 共有5090条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Cross-domain semantic priming of person recognition (from face primes to name targets at 500msecs SOA) is investigated in normal subjects and a brain-injured patient (PH) with a very severe impairment of overt face recognition ability. Experiment 1 demonstrates equivalent semantic priming effects for normal subjects from face primes to name targets (cross-domain priming) and from name primes to name targets (within-domain priming). Experiment 2 demonstrates cross-domain semantic priming effects from face primes that PH cannot recognize overtly. Experiment 3 shows that cross-domain semantic priming effects can be found for normal subjects when target names are repeated across all conditions. This (repeated targets) method is then used in Experiment 4 to establish that PH shows equivalent semantic priming to normal subjects from face primes which he is very poor at identifying overtly and from name primes which he can identify overtly. These findings demonstrate that automatic aspects of face recognition can remain intact even when all sense of overt recognition has been lost.  相似文献   
992.
Summary Two experiments investigating access to occupations and names of familiar people are reported, in which the response requirements of occupation and name-categorization tasks were made equivalent. In Experiment 1 matching tasks were used, in which subjects were required to determine whether simultaneously presented pairs of faces, surnames, or full names were those of people with the same or different occupations (politician or nonpolitician) or with same or different first names (Michael or David). Experiment 2 required binary classification of individual faces or surnames in terms of the bearer's occupation (politician or nonpolitician) or first name (Michael or David). In both experiments responses to faces were faster in tasks involving access to occupations than in tasks involving access to first names, whereas for surnames there was no difference in reaction times between occupations and first names in matching or classification tasks. These findings are consistent with the idea that identity-specific semantic codes and name codes are accessed sequentially from faces, but in parallel from written names.  相似文献   
993.
994.
The effects of morning, rush hour commuting were examined in a quasi-experimental field study involving government-employed commuters (single and carpool drivers). Commuting stress was measured as a response to variations in how difficult it was for commuters to move from home to work. The mediation of these effects by two sources of control in the commuting situation also were assessed. Control was operationalized as control over the internal environment of the car (single versus carpool driver) and choice over routes taken to get to work. Among commuters with a high impedance route, driving to work was associated with significant increases in systolic and diastolic blood pressure and decreases in behavioral performance. The stress of commuting under high impedance conditions was reduced for single drivers relative to carpoolers. Under similar conditions of high impedance, however, having the option to select more than one route to get to work seemed to be more stressful than having only one route. The practical implications of these results are discussed.  相似文献   
995.
Two kinds of measures of multivariate association, based on Wilks' and the Bartlett-Nanda-Pillai trace criterionV, respectively, are compared in terms of properties of the univariateR 2 which they generalize. A unified set of derivations of the properties is provided which are self-contained and not restricted to decompositions in canonical variates. One conclusion is that asymmetric index based on allows generalization of the multiplicative decomposition ofR 2 in terms of squared partial correlations, but not the additive decomposition in terms of squared semipartial correlations, while the reverse is true for anasymmetric index based onV.We are indebted to Jos M. F. ten Berge for some fruitful discussions.  相似文献   
996.
Stimulus-response compatibility in the programming of speech   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Subjects chose between sequences of one syllable (e.g.,/gi/vs./bi/), two syllables (e.g.,/gibi/ vs./gubu/), and three syllables (e.g.,/gibidi/ vs. gubudu/), when/i/sequences were signaled by high-pitched tones and/u] sequences were signaled by low-pitched tones (high compatibility), or the reverse (low compatibility). Choice times were additively affected by sequence length and compatibility. A second experiment showed attenuated compatibility effects for sequences with different vowels in the first and second syllables. These results replicate previously reported results for choices between finger sequences, which suggests that the same programming methods are used in both output domains. Evidently, choices between response sequences can be achieved by selecting a distinguishing parameter and assigning it in a serial fashion to partially prepared motor subprograms.  相似文献   
997.
Results are presented from a continuing study of men who graduated in 1970. Their first jobs after graduation were all typical of people with their degrees (in science, technology and engineering), but by 1977 they were in jobs which were identified as involving some degree of occupational change. These changes, and some of the beliefs the respondents held about them, are discussed. Tentative conclusions are drawn, and some possible implications for careers guidance are noted.  相似文献   
998.
Cognitive functioning and anxiety   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Summary Various possible differences in cognitive functioning between those high and low in trait anxiety are considered. Particular emphasis is paid to the hypothesis that individuals high in trait anxiety tend to approach threatening stimuli, whereas those low in trait anxiety tend to avoid such stimuli. The evidence indicates that there are such differences in the processing of threatening stimuli as a function of trait anxiety. However, these differences are found only under certain conditions, for example, when threatening and nonthreatening stimuli are presented concurrently, and when minor rather than major threat is involved.The differences between those high and low in trait anxiety encompass pre-attentive, attentional, and interpretative mechanisms. As a consequence, any adequate theory of trait anxiety must take proper account of cognitive mechanisms and functioning.  相似文献   
999.
This study systematically investigates changes in CNV waveform shape and resolution time that result from the presentation of facilitatory, inhibitory, or no motor response (MR) information simultaneously with the warning (S1) or imperative (S2) stimulus of the S1-S2-MR CNV paradigm. Analyses indicate that the simultaneous presentation of S1 and information to produce or inhibit a MR attenuates initial CNV development. Further, when the S1 information is inhibitive, CNV development is retarded throughout. The contribution of an inhibitory psychological process during CNV development is proposed. The data also indicate that CNV resolution time is not dependent on the presence of a motor response. It is suggested that CNV resolution time is indicative of psychological completion or closure.  相似文献   
1000.
Study 1 examined the hypothesis that ego-involvement leads to positive self-schema activation. Ego-involvement was induced by having experimental subjects anticipate a difficult test of intelligence. Noninvolved control subjects did not anticipate the test. All subjects completed a depth-of-processing task, following which incidental recall was assessed. Ego-involved relative to noninvolved subjects tended to recall a greater number of positive and fewer negative words at both the self-referent and semantic processing levels. Study 2 was conducted to address further the self-schema hypothesis and to address the hypothesis suggested by the results of Study 1 that ego-involvement leads to a positivity bias in information processing. The design of Study 2 was similar to that of Study 1. Results revealed that the self-favorability of negative words recalled at the self-referent level was greater for ego-involved than for noninvolved subjects, suggesting positive self-schema activation. In addition, ego-involved subjects recalled a greater number of positive words at the semantic level, suggesting a positivity bias in information processing.Study 2 was conducted by Andrew Howell in partial fulfillment of the requirements of a Master of Arts degree at Concordia University, under the supervision of Michael Conway. The authors thank Karin Stiefenhofer for her work on the project. The research was supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada and the Fonds pour la Formation de Chercheurs et l'Aide à la Recherche du Québec grants to Michael Conway.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号