全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3667篇 |
免费 | 247篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
3916篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 41篇 |
2022年 | 31篇 |
2021年 | 44篇 |
2020年 | 95篇 |
2019年 | 131篇 |
2018年 | 140篇 |
2017年 | 161篇 |
2016年 | 149篇 |
2015年 | 106篇 |
2014年 | 142篇 |
2013年 | 487篇 |
2012年 | 188篇 |
2011年 | 217篇 |
2010年 | 118篇 |
2009年 | 138篇 |
2008年 | 190篇 |
2007年 | 133篇 |
2006年 | 145篇 |
2005年 | 128篇 |
2004年 | 142篇 |
2003年 | 123篇 |
2002年 | 110篇 |
2001年 | 44篇 |
2000年 | 49篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 41篇 |
1997年 | 39篇 |
1996年 | 37篇 |
1995年 | 23篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 27篇 |
1991年 | 21篇 |
1990年 | 21篇 |
1989年 | 21篇 |
1988年 | 19篇 |
1987年 | 25篇 |
1986年 | 27篇 |
1985年 | 24篇 |
1984年 | 31篇 |
1983年 | 33篇 |
1982年 | 22篇 |
1981年 | 16篇 |
1980年 | 27篇 |
1979年 | 21篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 20篇 |
1976年 | 17篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有3916条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
32.
Brendan Gail Rule Ronald Dyck Andrew R. Nesdale 《European journal of social psychology》1978,8(2):237-244
The purpose of the present research was to assess the relative influence of instigation or inhibition in mediating retaliation after arbitrary or non-arbitrary frustration. Sixty-four men were asked to learn a concept that was being taught by a peer. Although all men were frustrated, half were deliberately frustrated while the other half were not deliberately frustrated by the teacher. Following the task, the learner was given the opportunity to prevent the teacher from gaining employment under conditions where his evaluation was either anonymous or to be made public. The results of a 2 × 2 analysis of variance yielded several significant effects. The data revealed that subjects rejected the teacher more when the frustration was arbitrary than when it was non-arbitrary. Moreover, when the evaluation was anonymous, subjects rejected the teacher more than when the evaluation was to be made public. Furthermore, the difference in the amount of rejection expressed between arbitrary and non-arbitrary conditions was greater when the evaluation was anonymous than when it was public. These results were interpreted as demonstrating the greater contribution of instigatory rather than inhibitory factors in expressing aggression following deliberate or non-deliberate frustration. 相似文献
33.
34.
Andrew F. Bambrick 《Contemporary Family Therapy》1980,2(2):99-105
Science asks that a theory state its basic terms and operations with economy and precision. Twenty-two terms and operations of Zuk's Go-Between Process are defined. It is desirable in science also to carefully distinguish among related theories. Go-Between Process and Structural-Strategic Family Therapy are related theories, and differences are described. 相似文献
35.
Robert Barcus Andrew I. Schwebel Samuel A. Corson 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1980,15(4):183-187
This report describes the use of an animal model for testing the relationship between the effects of ingesting food additives and hyperactivity. Telomian × beagle hybrid dogs were chosen as subjects because this breed has been observed to exhibit behavior seen as analogous to the hyperactivity of children. Two studies were conducted by adding or deleting butylated hydroxyanisole and Food, Drug, and Cosmetic red dye number 40 to the diet of each dog. A test of sitting was performed to measure the level of activity. While a relationship was not observed between the ingestion of the additive and the level of activity, several issues pertaining to the use of the animal model became apparent. These issues and the use of animals for future research in this area are discussed. 相似文献
36.
The communication disorder in dialysis dementia is an integral feature of the syndrome. This report describes the communication disorder in a 67-year-old man over a period of 4 months from his admission to the hospital to his death. In the early stage of the syndrome, the most prominent feature was a speech problem, the characteristics of which may vary among cases. In the middle stage, it was difficult to distinguish among aphemia, aphasia, or a combination of dysarthria and confusional state. The severity of the problems in these stages was temporally related to the dialysis treatments. The final stage was characterized by constant mutism and was unrelated to the dialysis treatments. Death occurred 10 months after the communication problems emerged. Communication studies of dialysis dementia patients may offer a systematic method to monitor the course of the syndrome, its response to therapeutic trials, and a clinical model for the study of communication disorders in general. 相似文献
37.
A left-handed patient with a right thalamic hemorrhage and disordered speech is described. Sequential examinations and aphasia testing were done during a 1-year follow-up period and the results are reported. This case supports those authors who have described characteristics they feel are helpful in diagnosing disordered speech associated with thalamic lesions. Paucity of speech, reduced voice volume, anomia, some paraphasia, and severe dysgraphia were present, but comprehension and repetition were relatively preserved. She showed modest improvement with time. This case also confirms that thalamic involvement in speech is a dominant, rather than a specifically left hemispheric function. 相似文献
38.
39.
Hypotheses linking a negative recall bias (or negative self-schema) with vulnerability to depression were tested using an experimental mood-induction task. Extent of bias in student subjects was found to be unstable over a 4-month period, but different measures of memory bias within one session were highly correlated. Marginally significant correlations in the expected direction were found between estimates of recall bias and mood state, whether measured at the same or at different times. Change in mood following induction did not relate to change in bias, suggesting that the observed differences in recall are not a simple function of transient mood state alone. Equally, the initial recall bias did not predict the extent of mood shift following induction, as would be expected if a negative self-schema determined ease of mood shift in a negative direction. However, significant correlations were found between negative recall bias and self-reported frequency and severity of depression, lending equivocal support to the negative self-schema model. 相似文献
40.
The two studies undertaken investigated how reaction times are affected by preknowledge concerning hand and direction parameters, and the order in which these data are disclosed. In the first experiment, preknowledge was manipulated by selecting 2-CRT pairs which had either a common hand or direction component. Reaction signal information content (as defined by the number of unknown response parameters) was controlled, but no parameter differences emerged. When parameter order was manipulated by sequential precuing, the ‘hand then direction’ order conferred a significant advantage. Motor preparation and response selection explanations for this effect are considered. Various difficulties with the paradigms used are discussed, including those of distinguishing between response selection and motor programming contributions, controlling and inferring the strategies adopted by subjects, and defining task difficulty. The prospects for independently fostering either response identification or motor preparation by means of an appropriate selection of precues are also discussed. 相似文献