首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   174629篇
  免费   7876篇
  国内免费   158篇
  2021年   1498篇
  2020年   2793篇
  2019年   3464篇
  2018年   3560篇
  2017年   4014篇
  2016年   4643篇
  2015年   3939篇
  2014年   4813篇
  2013年   23555篇
  2012年   4647篇
  2011年   3812篇
  2010年   3927篇
  2009年   4796篇
  2008年   3960篇
  2007年   3510篇
  2006年   4120篇
  2005年   4078篇
  2004年   3603篇
  2003年   3193篇
  2002年   3027篇
  2001年   3281篇
  2000年   3086篇
  1999年   3099篇
  1998年   2827篇
  1997年   2660篇
  1996年   2577篇
  1995年   2416篇
  1994年   2379篇
  1993年   2326篇
  1992年   2553篇
  1991年   2399篇
  1990年   2260篇
  1989年   2212篇
  1988年   2184篇
  1987年   2215篇
  1986年   2179篇
  1985年   2430篇
  1984年   2524篇
  1983年   2330篇
  1982年   2383篇
  1981年   2343篇
  1980年   2198篇
  1979年   2212篇
  1978年   2182篇
  1977年   2144篇
  1976年   1958篇
  1975年   2000篇
  1974年   2047篇
  1973年   1954篇
  1972年   1528篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
In this study, we explored the structure of pupils’ creativity in primary education following the Amusement Park Theory, by investigating undiscovered linkages between the domains of writing, mathematics, and drawing. More specifically, we examined: (a) whether some domains and general thematic areas are more closely related to each other than to others, (b) whether literacy and mathematical ability are specific underlying traits of creativity in writing and mathematics, respectively, and (c) whether intelligence and divergent thinking are related to creativity in all domains. The sample consisted of 331 Dutch 4th grade pupils. For each research question, a model was analyzed using structural equation modeling. We found creativity in mathematics and creativity in writing to be most similar, followed by creativity in mathematics and creativity in drawing, with creativity in writing and creativity in drawing being least similar. Additionally, we found evidence for several underlying traits (i.e., literacy ability and mathematical ability) and initial requirements of creativity (i.e., intelligence and divergent thinking), none of which were important for creativity in only one domain, and of which only intelligence was important for creativity in all domains. Herewith, our study provides insights regarding the complexity of the structure of creativity in primary education.  相似文献   
172.
173.
Intention is theorized as the proximal determinant of behavior in some theories of motivation, but the need to understand predictors of action control (i.e., translating an intention into behavior) is warranted to tailor physical activity intervention efforts. The purpose of this study was to examine constructs of the transtheoretical model of behavior change (TTM) as predictors of physical activity intention-behavior profiles across 6 months in a large Canadian sample (N = 1,192). Results showed that 5 of the 8 possible intention-behavior profiles had a substantial number of participants: nonintenders, unsuccessful adopters, successful adopters, unsuccessful maintainers, and successful maintainers. Constructs of the TTM distinguished (p < .01) intention-behavior profiles. Self-efficacy and the behavioral processes of change were particularly good predictors of action control (p < .01), but disaggregated beliefs and processes identified specific intervention targets for successful physical activity adoption and maintenance. The results validate that both action planning and action control are important when understanding physical activity behavior.  相似文献   
174.
175.
176.
177.
178.
Two experiments examined repetition priming in the recognition of famous voices. In Experiment 1, reaction times for fame decisions to famous voice samples were shorter than in an unprimed condition, when voices were primed by a different voice sample of the same person having been presented in an earlier phase of the experiment. No effect of voice repetition was observed for non-famous voices. In Experiment 2, it was investigated whether this priming effect is voice-specific or whether it is related to post-perceptual processes in person recognition. Recognizing a famous voice was again primed by having earlier heard a different voice sample of that person. Although an earlier exposure to that person's name did not cause any priming, there was some indication of priming following an earlier exposure to that person's face. Finally, earlier exposure to the identical voice sample (as compared to a different voice sample from the same person) caused a considerable bias towards responding 'famous'-i.e. performance benefits for famous but costs for nonfamous voices. The findings suggestthat (1) repetition priming invoice recognition primarily involves the activation of perceptual representations of voices, and (2) it is important to determine the conditions in which priming causes bias effects that need to be disentangled from performance benefits.  相似文献   
179.
180.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号