全文获取类型
收费全文 | 24356篇 |
免费 | 68篇 |
专业分类
24424篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 25篇 |
2023年 | 43篇 |
2022年 | 33篇 |
2021年 | 49篇 |
2020年 | 101篇 |
2019年 | 142篇 |
2018年 | 3595篇 |
2017年 | 2953篇 |
2016年 | 2387篇 |
2015年 | 306篇 |
2014年 | 234篇 |
2013年 | 585篇 |
2012年 | 749篇 |
2011年 | 2587篇 |
2010年 | 2611篇 |
2009年 | 1604篇 |
2008年 | 1884篇 |
2007年 | 2297篇 |
2006年 | 182篇 |
2005年 | 344篇 |
2004年 | 307篇 |
2003年 | 244篇 |
2002年 | 174篇 |
2001年 | 78篇 |
2000年 | 103篇 |
1999年 | 44篇 |
1998年 | 63篇 |
1997年 | 58篇 |
1996年 | 45篇 |
1995年 | 25篇 |
1994年 | 27篇 |
1993年 | 19篇 |
1992年 | 31篇 |
1991年 | 30篇 |
1990年 | 30篇 |
1989年 | 24篇 |
1988年 | 22篇 |
1987年 | 27篇 |
1986年 | 31篇 |
1985年 | 27篇 |
1984年 | 31篇 |
1983年 | 37篇 |
1982年 | 23篇 |
1981年 | 19篇 |
1980年 | 30篇 |
1979年 | 22篇 |
1978年 | 21篇 |
1977年 | 20篇 |
1976年 | 18篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
821.
Johanna Matikainen 《Journal of International Migration and Integration》2003,4(2):273-295
Finland has quite a short history of receiving refugees. Compared to the other western countries, Finland has few immigrants
living within its borders: they represent less than 2% of the population. The Finnish government has the responsibility of
receiving refugees. Every immigrant who registers as an unemployed job seeker is entitled to a personal integration plan.
The role of the NGOs is to supplement the elements and opportunities of these integration plans. What this system needs is
the establishment of cooperation between the authorities, the NGOs and the immigrants. The Finnish Red Cross “Kotopolku” project
assists the immigrants in their integration process and creates new tools for integration. “The Integration-Timeline” is a
tool based on life span theories which aims to assist immigrants in analysing their own integration process in order to be
able to make better plans and life strategies for themselves. 相似文献
822.
823.
Weaver AJ Flannelly KJ Koenig HG Smith FD 《The journal of pastoral care & counseling : JPCC》2004,58(4):343-350
Based on the content analysis of quantitative research appearing in three medical journals, the authors conclude that, despite the shared ideal of providing spiritual care to patients on the part of physicians and chaplains, there is little attention given in these journals demonstrating and promoting this shared perspective. Suggestions for future research that would focus on this common medicine/religion interface and concern are noted. 相似文献
824.
Kurland J Naeser MA Baker EH Doron K Martin PI Seekins HE Bogdan A Renshaw P Yurgelun-Todd D 《Behavioural neurology》2004,15(3-4):87-97
Cortical reorganization in poststroke aphasia is not well understood. Few studies have investigated neural mechanisms underlying language recovery in severe aphasia patients, who are typically viewed as having a poor prognosis for language recovery. Although test-retest reliability is routinely demonstrated during collection of language data in single-subject aphasia research, this is rarely examined in fMRI studies investigating the underlying neural mechanisms in aphasia recovery. The purpose of this study was to acquire fMRI test-retest data examining semantic decisions both within and between two aphasia patients. Functional MRI was utilized to image individuals with chronic, moderate-severe nonfluent aphasia during nonverbal, yes/no button-box semantic judgments of iconic sentences presented in the Computer-assisted Visual Communication (C-ViC) program. We investigated the critical issue of intra-subject reliability by exploring similarities and differences in regions of activation during participants' performance of identical tasks twice on the same day. Each participant demonstrated high intra-subject reliability, with response decrements typical of task familiarity. Differences between participants included greater left hemisphere perilesional activation in the individual with better response to C-ViC training. This study provides fMRI reliability in chronic nonfluent aphasia, and adds to evidence supporting differences in individual cortical reorganization in aphasia recovery. 相似文献
825.
Schaie KW Caskie GI Revell AJ Willis SL Kaszniak AW Teri L 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2005,12(3):245-277
A battery of 17 neuropsychological tests (including the CERAD battery) and 17 psychometric ability tests were administered to a sample of 499 participants of the Seattle Longitudinal study who had been given the psychometric ability tests seven and 14 years earlier. The neuropsychological tests were projected into a 5-factor psychometric ability space by means of extension analysis. The concurrent regressions of the neuropsychology tests on the psychometric ability tests were then used to estimate neuropsychology test scores from the psychometric ability tests administered in 1984, 1991 and 1998. Neuropsychologists then rated the study participants as either normal, suspect or cognitively impaired in 1998. Changes in estimated test scores were computed over seven and fourteen years. Significant odds ratios between normal and cognitively impaired groups were found for all neuropsychological tests over the proximal period and for most tests over the 14-year period. Similar findings occurred for the odds ratios between the normal and suspect groups for the most proximal 7-year changes. 相似文献
826.
The relationship between online visual representation of a scene and long-term scene memory 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Hollingworth A 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2005,31(3):396-411
In 3 experiments the author investigated the relationship between the online visual representation of natural scenes and long-term visual memory. In a change detection task, a target object either changed or remained the same from an initial image of a natural scene to a test image. Two types of changes were possible: rotation in depth, or replacement by another object from the same basic-level category. Change detection during online scene viewing was compared with change detection after delay of 1 trial (Experiments 2A and 2B) until the end of the study session (Experiment 1) or 24 hr (Experiment 3). There was little or no decline in change detection performance from online viewing to a delay of 1 trial or delay until the end of the session, and change detection remained well above chance after 24 hr. These results demonstrate that long-term memory for visual detail in a scene is robust. 相似文献
827.
Neurons were found in the rhesus macaque anterior orbitofrontal cortex that respond to novel but not to familiar visual stimuli. Some of these neurons responded to all novel stimuli, and others to only a subset (e.g., to novel faces). The neurons have no responses to familiar reward- or punishment-associated visual stimuli, nor to taste, olfactory or somatosensory inputs. The responses of the neurons typically habituated with repeated presentations of a novel stimulus, and five presentations each 1s was the median number for the response to reach half-maximal. The neurons did not respond to stimuli which had been novel and shown a few times on the previous day, indicating that the neurons were involved in long-term memory. The median latency of the neuronal responses was 120 ms. The median spontaneous firing rate was 1.3 spikes/s, and the median response to novel visual stimuli was 6.0 spikes/s. These findings indicate that the long-term memory for visual stimuli is information that is represented in a region of the primate anterior orbitofrontal cortex. 相似文献
828.
Mohanty A Herrington JD Koven NS Fisher JE Wenzel EA Webb AG Heller W Banich MT Miller GA 《Journal of abnormal psychology》2005,114(1):16-27
Negatively valenced stimuli foster cognitive impairment in schizotypy and schizophrenia. To identify relevant brain mechanisms, the authors had 16 positive-schizotypy and 16 control participants perform an emotional Stroop task, judging the ink color of negative and neutral words during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) of regional brain activity. Schizotypy individuals showed increased right and decreased left activity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, indicating a deficit in maintenance of attentional set in the presence of negative emotional distractors. They also showed abnormal activity in ventral limbic areas, including decreased activity in nucleus accumbens and increased activity in hippocampus and amygdala, a circuit involved in the integration of cognitive and affective processes. These results indicate that aspects of emotion-cognition processes and the brain mechanisms that implement them are similar in schizotypy and schizophrenia. 相似文献
829.
Eric Marcus 《Philosophical Studies》2005,122(1):27-50
It is generally accepted that the most serious threat to the possibility of mental causation is posed by the causal self-sufficiency of physical causal processes. I argue, however, that this feature of the world, which I articulate in principle I call Completeness, in fact poses no genuine threat to mental causation. Some find Completeness threatening to mental causation because they confuse it with a stronger principle, which I call Closure. Others do not simply conflate Completeness and Closure, but hold that Completeness, together with certain plausible assumptions, entails Closure. I refute the most fully worked-out version of such an argument. Finally, some find Completeness all by itself threatening to mental causation. I argue that one will only find Completeness threatening if one operates with a philosophically distorted conception of mental causation. I thereby defend what I call naïve realism about mental causation. 相似文献
830.
Eugene Thomas Long 《International Journal for Philosophy of Religion》2005,57(3):219-220