全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1492篇 |
免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
1519篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 12篇 |
2023年 | 17篇 |
2022年 | 19篇 |
2021年 | 19篇 |
2020年 | 37篇 |
2019年 | 36篇 |
2018年 | 46篇 |
2017年 | 57篇 |
2016年 | 65篇 |
2015年 | 55篇 |
2014年 | 46篇 |
2013年 | 165篇 |
2012年 | 76篇 |
2011年 | 90篇 |
2010年 | 50篇 |
2009年 | 52篇 |
2008年 | 68篇 |
2007年 | 61篇 |
2006年 | 47篇 |
2005年 | 39篇 |
2004年 | 39篇 |
2003年 | 32篇 |
2002年 | 34篇 |
2001年 | 27篇 |
2000年 | 30篇 |
1999年 | 22篇 |
1998年 | 12篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 13篇 |
1991年 | 16篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1970年 | 7篇 |
1969年 | 7篇 |
1968年 | 5篇 |
1967年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有1519条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
231.
Framing effects on emotional responses in a green electricity choice are investigated in a survey of a random sample of 655 Swedish residents. The results show that overall more positive and less negative emotions are experienced for a promotion frame (“choosing green electricity and paying more”) than for a prevention frame (“not choosing green electricity and paying less”). Value orientation is shown to moderate the framing effect in that for the promotion frame, individuals with a self‐transcendence value orientation show the most positive and least negative emotion, whereas for the prevention frame, individuals with a self‐enhancement value orientation show the most positive and least negative emotion. Emotional responses are further shown to mediate the relationship between value orientation and intention to purchase eco‐labeled electricity. 相似文献
232.
233.
234.
235.
Social skills training for children is becoming increasingly popular as a measure for developmental crime prevention. Although previous reviews of such programs have shown positive effects, they have also revealed problems of research design, outcome measures, and long-term follow up. Accordingly, this article reports on a recent meta-analysis of randomized evaluations of the effect of social skills training in preventing antisocial behavior and promoting social competence. Of 841 retrievable references, 84 research reports with a total of 136 treatment-control comparisons fulfilled the eligibility criteria. Results showed a small but significant overall positive effect of d = .39 at post-intervention and d = .28 at follow-up (3 months and later). Effect sizes were somewhat greater for outcome measures of social competence than for measures of antisocial behavior, particularly when delinquency was assessed. Cognitive-behavioral programs revealed the best results in terms of generalization over time and on outcome criteria. In addition, prevention measures indicated for children and adolescents who already manifested some behavioral problems had higher effect sizes than universal approaches. Because most studies dealt with small sample sizes, non-official outcome data, and measurements after less than one year, the results should be interpreted with caution. Further high-quality studies with long-term empirical outcome criteria are needed, particularly outside the United States. 相似文献
236.
Schneider KJ 《The American psychologist》2006,61(3):258; discussion 259-258; discussion 261
237.
Gerhard Blickle Paula B. Schneider James A. Meurs Pamela L. Perrewé 《Journal of applied social psychology》2010,40(8):1897-1920
Mentoring is prototypically intended to advance the personal and professional growth of new employees at work. Although meta‐analyses have found that receiving mentoring can result in beneficial outcomes for employees' career success, employees may perceive barriers to obtaining a mentor. The present research examined antecedents and consequences to perceived barriers to mentoring in business and administrative jobs in a field study over 2 years. Socioeconomic origin, positive affectivity, organizational development culture, and previous mentoring experience predicted perceived barriers to mentoring after 2 years. New employees' perceived barriers to mentoring at Time 1 predicted changes in mentoring received and income after 2 years. Implications of this study, including a proposed mentoring training program, and directions for future research are discussed. 相似文献
238.
Andreas Vrahimis 《British Journal for the History of Philosophy》2013,21(1):177-188
In his recent article, titled ‘Royaumont Revisited’, Overgaard challenges Dummett's view that one needs to go as far back as the late nineteenth century in order to discover examples of genuine dialogue between ‘analytic’ and ‘continental’ philosophy. Instead, Overgaard argues that in the 1958 Royaumont colloquium, generally judged as a failed attempt at communication between the two camps, one can find some elements which may be utilized towards re-establishing a dialogue between these two sides. Yet, emphasising this image of Royaumont as a kind of battleground between ‘analytic’ and ‘continental’ philosophy obscures the plurality of philosophical approaches involved. Royaumont was the meeting point of more than two philosophical traditions, as can be shown by the divergent interests of its participants. Thus, though the potential for rapprochement between Oxford ‘linguistic philosophy’ and a certain strand of phenomenological thought may indeed be found among the discussions that took place during the colloquium, one should keep in mind that such rapprochement took place in the context of a meeting between, among others: continental ‘analytics’, Anglophone non-‘analytics’, French historians of philosophy, ‘analytic’ opponents of Oxford philosophy, Franciscan phenomenologists, and Oxonians who called their work ‘phenomenology’. 相似文献
239.
Selective retrieval of some studied items can both impair and improve recall of the other items. This study examined the role of working memory capacity (WMC) for the two effects of memory retrieval. Participants studied an item list consisting of predefined target and nontarget items. After study of the list, half of the participants performed an imagination task supposed to induce a change in mental context, whereas the other half performed a counting task which does not induce such context change. Following presentation of a second list, memory for the original list's target items was tested, either with or without preceding retrieval of the list's nontarget items. Consistent with previous work, preceding nontarget retrieval impaired target recall in the absence of the context change, but improved target recall in its presence. In particular, there was a positive relationship between WMC and the beneficial, but not the detrimental effect of memory retrieval. On the basis of the view that the beneficial effect of memory retrieval reflects context-reactivation processes, the results indicate that individuals with higher WMC are better able to capitalise on retrieval-induced context reactivation than individuals with lower WMC. 相似文献
240.