全文获取类型
收费全文 | 799篇 |
免费 | 16篇 |
专业分类
815篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 10篇 |
2023年 | 11篇 |
2022年 | 16篇 |
2021年 | 15篇 |
2020年 | 23篇 |
2019年 | 24篇 |
2018年 | 35篇 |
2017年 | 34篇 |
2016年 | 45篇 |
2015年 | 36篇 |
2014年 | 27篇 |
2013年 | 112篇 |
2012年 | 57篇 |
2011年 | 59篇 |
2010年 | 26篇 |
2009年 | 23篇 |
2008年 | 46篇 |
2007年 | 37篇 |
2006年 | 25篇 |
2005年 | 21篇 |
2004年 | 25篇 |
2003年 | 15篇 |
2002年 | 16篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1962年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有815条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
621.
Carlos Ventura-Bort Andreas Löw Julia Wendt Javier Moltó Rosario Poy Florin Dolcos Alfons O. Hamm Mathias Weymar 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2016,16(2):234-247
There is abundant evidence in memory research that emotional stimuli are better remembered than neutral stimuli. However, effects of an emotionally charged context on memory for associated neutral elements is also important, particularly in trauma and stress-related disorders, where strong memories are often activated by neutral cues due to their emotional associations. In the present study, we used event-related potentials (ERPs) to investigate long-term recognition memory (1-week delay) for neutral objects that had been paired with emotionally arousing or neutral scenes during encoding. Context effects were clearly evident in the ERPs: An early frontal ERP old/new difference (300–500 ms) was enhanced for objects encoded in unpleasant compared to pleasant and neutral contexts; and a late central-parietal old/new difference (400–700 ms) was observed for objects paired with both pleasant and unpleasant contexts but not for items paired with neutral backgrounds. Interestingly, objects encoded in emotional contexts (and novel objects) also prompted an enhanced frontal early (180–220 ms) positivity compared to objects paired with neutral scenes indicating early perceptual significance. The present data suggest that emotional—particularly unpleasant—backgrounds strengthen memory for items encountered within these contexts and engage automatic and explicit recognition processes. These results could help in understanding binding mechanisms involved in the activation of trauma-related memories by neutral cues. 相似文献
622.
The belief in paranormal phenomena is a frequently studied topic. Studies predominantly assess explicit (i.e., conscious) parts of paranormal belief (PB) using questionnaire-based self-report measures which are prone to impression management and social desirability tendencies. In order to investigate the usefulness of measuring implicit (i.e., automatic) PB, we developed a PB Implicit Association Test (PB-IAT). Implicit PB was uncorrelated with explicit PB, but moderated the relationship between explicit PB and participants’ knowledge of paranormal phenomena. Participants with a weak implicit PB did not differ in their knowledge scores regardless of whether they had strong or weak explicit PB. But participants with strong implicit PB had higher scores when they also had strong explicit PB compared to participants with weak explicit PB. These results suggest that discrepant configurations of PB impair performance in a knowledge test about paranormal phenomena. 相似文献
623.
Peter Fischer Tobias Greitemeyer Andreas Kastenmüller Dieter Frey Silvia Oßwald 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2007,43(6):964-971
In recent times, international terrorism has become one of the most serious concerns of the modern Western world. As a consequence, the risk and fear of terrorism is more salient in people’s minds (i.e. terror salience), which is supposed to affect behavioral responses in our society. The present research suggests that increased punishment of violations of criminal laws is one societal reaction to increased terror salience. It was consistently shown that increased terror salience, induced by either varying the temporal distance from the London terrorist attacks of July 7, 2005 (Study 1), by vignettes in the form of newspaper articles manipulating the expected terror risk (Study 2), or by pictures of recent terrorist attacks (Studies 3 and 4), significantly increased the punishment assigned to a violation of criminal laws that was completely unrelated to terror (i.e. car theft and rape). Finally, Study 4 revealed that (a) the effect of terror salience on punishment is indeed due to increased perception of threat to social order and (b) that this effect cannot be explained by the general negative nature of the stimulus material employed in this study. 相似文献
624.
This explorative study examines the effects of an internet-based cognitive-behavioural therapy for complicated grief on post-traumatic growth and optimism. The study is part of a larger randomized controlled trial described in Wagner, Knaevelsrud, and Maercker (2006). The patients were randomly assigned to either a treatment group (n = 26) or a waiting list control condition (n = 25). The internet-based intervention consisted of exposure to bereavement cues, cognitive reappraisal exercises, and a module on integration and restoration. A short form of the Post-traumatic Growth Inventory (PTGI), the Life Orientation Test-Revised (LOT-R), and measures of complicated grief and psychopathological outcomes were administered. Results indicate that post-traumatic growth increased in the treatment group. No treatment effect was found for optimism. These findings contribute to the growing literature on personal growth in psychotherapy. 相似文献
625.
The endogenous compound androstadienone modulating the evaluation of others and activating the human fear system was hypothesized in terms of processing socially relevant cues by regulating responses to angry faces. Androstadienone was investigated in association with arm movements of 62 participants (30 women) in response to happy and angry facial expressions. Volunteers pushed away or pulled toward them a joystick as fast as possible on seeing either an angry or a happy cartoon face on a computer screen. This task was repeated twice: once during exposure to androstadienone masked with clove oil and once to clove oil only. In the former condition participants' reaction speed was accelerated, especially when reacting to angry faces. This observation may indicate an androstadienone-related activation of the fear system leading to faster responses to threat signals, assuming an enhanced allocation of attentional resources toward threat-related social cues. 相似文献
626.
Christopher M. Warren Andreas T. Breuer Justin Kantner Daniel Fiset Caroline Blais Michael E. J. Masson 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2009,16(6):1106-1111
We provide evidence that the locus coeruleus—norephinephrine (LC-NE) system is the neurophysiological basis of the attentional
blink. The attentional blink refers to decreased accuracy for reporting the second of two targets in a rapid serial visual
presentation of distractors. The LC-NE account of the attentional blink posits that targets elicit a facilitative LC-NE system
response that is available for the first target but subsequently unavailable to the second, due to the autoinhibitory nature
of the LC-NE system. We propose a modification of the LC-NE account, suggesting that the LC-NE system response is elicited
by interference between mutually exclusive responses demanded by temporally proximal targets and distractors. We increased
the interference between the first target and the following distractor by reducing the time between them. For identifying
the second target this high-interference condition yielded a benefit up to 200 msec after onset of the first, followed by
a decrease in accuracy. Consistent with our modification of the LC-NE account, this result suggests a temporarily enhanced
LC-NE system response to increased target—distractor interference. 相似文献
627.
The present electromyographic study is a first step toward shedding light on the involvement of affective processes in congruent and incongruent facial reactions to facial expressions. Further, empathy was investigated as a potential mediator underlying the modulation of facial reactions to emotional faces in a competitive, a cooperative, and a neutral setting. Results revealed less congruent reactions to happy expressions and even incongruent reactions to sad and angry expressions in the competition condition, whereas virtually no differences between the neutral and the cooperation condition occurred. Effects on congruent reactions were found to be mediated by cognitive empathy, indicating that the state of empathy plays an important role in the situational modulation of congruent reactions. Further, incongruent reactions to sad and angry faces in a competition setting were mediated by the emotional reaction of joy, supporting the assumption that incongruent facial reactions are mainly based on affective processes. Additionally, strategic processes (specifically, the goal to create and maintain a smooth, harmonious interaction) were found to influence facial reactions while being in a cooperative mindset. Now, further studies are needed to test for the generalizability of these effects. 相似文献
628.
The honey bee is a model organism for studies on the neural substrates of learning and memory. Associative olfactory learning
using sucrose rewards is fast and reliable in foragers and older hive bees. However, researchers have so far failed to show
any significant learning in newly emerged bees. It is generally argued that in these bees only part of the brain structures
important for learning are fully developed. Here we show for the first time that newly emerged honey bees are capable of associative
learning, if they are sufficiently responsive to sucrose. Responsiveness to sucrose, which can be measured using the proboscis
extension response (PER), increases with age. Newly emerged bees are on average very unresponsive to sucrose. We show that
if newly emerged bees displaying a PER to 10% sucrose or lower sucrose concentrations are conditioned to an odour, they show
significant associative learning and early long-term memory. Nevertheless, the level of acquisition is still lower than in
foragers. The general assumption that newly emerged honey bees are incapable of associative learning must therefore be reconsidered.
Further, our study suggests that an age-dependent increase in responsiveness to rewarding stimuli is directly related to the
development of early learning abilities. The decisive influence of responsiveness to rewarding stimuli in associative learning
of newly emerged bees has far reaching consequences for studies on the development of associative learning capabilities in
insects and vertebrates. 相似文献
629.
Mario Gollwitzer Tobias Rothmund Andreas Pfeiffer Conrad Ensenbach 《Journal of research in personality》2009,43(6):999-1005
People differ in how injustice-sensitive they are either as victims or as observers. Whereas observer sensitivity is positively related to cooperative behavior, victim sensitivity promotes antisocial and egoistic behavior. The present article investigates the dynamics underlying these effects. Participants played an online-based public goods game and were informed about the number of people who violated a fairness rule in previous rounds of the game (no, some, or many violators). High victim-sensitive participants contributed less to the public good even in the “some violators” condition. High observer-sensitive participants contributed more to the public good even in the “many violators” condition. The findings correspond with the sensitivity to mean intentions model and cannot be explained by individual differences in general trust. 相似文献
630.