全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3143篇 |
免费 | 63篇 |
专业分类
3206篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 17篇 |
2023年 | 21篇 |
2022年 | 21篇 |
2021年 | 33篇 |
2020年 | 62篇 |
2019年 | 58篇 |
2018年 | 107篇 |
2017年 | 135篇 |
2016年 | 131篇 |
2015年 | 92篇 |
2014年 | 118篇 |
2013年 | 381篇 |
2012年 | 166篇 |
2011年 | 175篇 |
2010年 | 84篇 |
2009年 | 72篇 |
2008年 | 143篇 |
2007年 | 137篇 |
2006年 | 114篇 |
2005年 | 112篇 |
2004年 | 110篇 |
2003年 | 80篇 |
2002年 | 110篇 |
2001年 | 43篇 |
2000年 | 48篇 |
1999年 | 40篇 |
1998年 | 33篇 |
1997年 | 29篇 |
1996年 | 26篇 |
1995年 | 20篇 |
1994年 | 23篇 |
1993年 | 20篇 |
1992年 | 29篇 |
1991年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 17篇 |
1988年 | 20篇 |
1987年 | 19篇 |
1985年 | 23篇 |
1984年 | 24篇 |
1983年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 18篇 |
1980年 | 18篇 |
1979年 | 16篇 |
1978年 | 18篇 |
1977年 | 21篇 |
1976年 | 17篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
1968年 | 18篇 |
1967年 | 17篇 |
排序方式: 共有3206条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
The effect of school-based kindergarten transition policies and practices on child academic outcomes
This study examined the effect of school-based kindergarten transition policies and practices on child outcomes. The authors followed 17,212 children from 992 schools in the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Kindergarten sample (ECLS-K) across the kindergarten school year. Hierarchical linear modeling revealed that the number of school-based transition practices in the fall of kindergarten was associated with more positive academic achievement scores at the end of kindergarten, even controlling for family socioeconomic status (SES) and other demographic factors. This effect was stronger for low- and middle-SES children than high-SES children. For low-SES children, 7 transition practices were associated with a .21 standard deviation increase in predicted achievement scores beyond 0 practices. The effect of transition practices was partially mediated by an intervening effect on parent-initiated involvement in school during the kindergarten year. The findings support education policies to target kindergarten transition efforts to increase parent involvement in low-SES families. 相似文献
92.
Students compared computer-generated pictures of which 1 had line spacing and orientation proportional to that in a Mondrian picture (Mondrian-like), and 9 had divergent line spacing (divergently spaced). Preference was above average for the Mondrian-like picture compared with that for the divergently spaced pictures. However, participants did not prefer the Mondrian-like picture to divergently spaced pictures that were preselected as aesthetically pleasing by other participants. The results of the present experiments suggest that a computer algorithm can be developed to modify a picture in a way that is comparable with the way in which humans modify a picture to obtain a preferred picture. If aesthetic appeal is used in the storage and retrieval of visual information, then the algorithm could provide a better understanding of human perceptual processes. The results are related to consistency of aesthetic judgments across participants, levels of processing, and apparent contrast effects. 相似文献
93.
Learmonth AE Lamberth R Rovee-Collier C 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2005,91(4):297-314
Infants increasingly generalize deferred imitation across environmental contexts between 6 and 18 months of age. In three experiments with 126 6-, 9-, 12-, 15-, and 18-month-olds, we examined the role of the social context in deferred imitation. One experimenter demonstrated target actions on a hand puppet, and a second experimenter tested imitation 24h later. When the second experimenter was novel, infants did not exhibit deferred imitation at any age; when infants were preexposed to the second experimenter, all of them did. Imitating immediately after the demonstration also facilitated deferred imitation in a novel social context at all ages but 6 months. Infants' pervasive failure to exhibit deferred imitation in a novel social context may reflect evolutionary selection pressures that favored conservative behavior in social animals. 相似文献
94.
This study meta-analytically examined extensive literature associated with work commitment. The primary purposes were to (a) cumulate correlations among dimensions of work commitment to see which were intercorrelated and (b) determine impact of work commitment dimensions and subdimensions on specific outcome variables (job satisfaction, job performance, turnover intentions, and turnover). Results were cumulated across 997 articles. The positive manifold of correlations suggests the presence of a common psychological construct underlying different commitment forms, with the exception of calculative, continuance, and union commitment. Most of the 94 meta-analyzed correlations were small, suggesting that concept redundancy is not a major concern. Meta-analyses of the correlations of 24 commitment constructs with 4 outcome variables suggest that different commitment forms have similar patterns of correlations with outcome variables. 相似文献
95.
We carried out three experiments to examine the influence of field of view and binocular viewing restrictions on absolute distance perception in real-world indoor environments. Few of the classical visual cues provide direct information for accurate absolute distance judgments to points in the environment beyond about 2 m from the viewer. Nevertheless, in previous work it has been found that visually directed walking tasks reveal accurate distance estimations in full-cue real-world environments to distances up to 20 m. In contrast, the same tasks in virtual environments produced with head-mounted displays (HMDs) show large compression of distance. Field of view and binocular viewing are common limitations in research with HMDs, and have been rarely studied under full pictorial-cue conditions in the context of distance perception in the real-world. Experiment 1 showed that the view of one's body and feet on the floor was not necessary for accurate distance perception. In experiment 2 we manipulated the horizontal and the vertical field of view along with head rotation and found that a restricted field of view did not affect the accuracy of distance estimations when head movement was allowed. Experiment 3 showed that performance with monocular viewing was equal to that with binocular viewing. These results have implications for the information needed to scale egocentric distance in the real-world and reduce the support for the hypothesis that a limited field of view or imperfections in binocular image presentation are the cause of the underestimation seen with HMDs. 相似文献
96.
Rosemary?FlanaganEmail author Korrie?Allen 《Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy》2005,23(3):263-273
A review and critique of the Children’s Inventory of Anger (ChIA) is presented. The ChIA is a 39-item measure that operationalizes
anger according to the conceptualization set forth by Ellis in Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy. The ChIA consists of four
subscales (Frustration, Physical Aggression, Peer Relationships, and Authority Relations) which provide information regarding
situations that are commonly associated with the experience of anger. Preliminary validity studies demonstrate that the ChIA
has excellent reliability and high face validity. The forms are user friendly and easily scored, with norms tables conveniently
located within the form. A computer-scoring program is available which significantly reduces clerical errors. The application
of the instrument in practice is discussed. Due to the fact that the ChIA attempts to link assessment and intervention it
should be useful in advancing the field in regard to treatment evaluation and substantiation of treatment validity. It is
also expected to have considerable research utility. 相似文献
97.
The hindsight bias (e.g., Fischhoff, 1975) illustrates that outcome information can make people believe that they would have (or did) predict an outcome that they would not (or did not) actually predict. In two experiments, participants (N = 226) made a prediction immediately before receiving outcome information. Therefore, participants could not distort or misremember their predictions to make them align with the outcome information. In both experiments, participants distorted their reports of how certain they recalled having been in their prediction, how good of a basis they had for making the prediction, how long they took to make the prediction, and so forth. Experiment 2 showed that these effects were diminished when participants engaged in private thought about the upcoming questions prior to receiving outcome information, suggesting that the effect is not due to impression management concerns. 相似文献
98.
Hall AL 《The American journal of bioethics : AJOB》2005,5(3):59-60; discussion W10-2
99.
Campbell AT 《The American journal of bioethics : AJOB》2005,5(6):66-8; discussion W14-6
100.
As the Internet has changed communication, commerce, and the distribution of information, so too it is changing psychological research. Psychologists can observe new or rare phenomena online and can do research on traditional psychological topics more efficiently, enabling them to expand the scale and scope of their research. Yet these opportunities entail risk both to research quality and to human subjects. Internet research is inherently no more risky than traditional observational, survey, or experimental methods. Yet the risks and safeguards against them will differ from those characterizing traditional research and will themselves change over time. This article describes some benefits and challenges of conducting psychological research via the Internet and offers recommendations to both researchers and institutional review boards for dealing with them. ((c) 2004 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献