全文获取类型
收费全文 | 763篇 |
免费 | 47篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
812篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 10篇 |
2019年 | 23篇 |
2018年 | 17篇 |
2017年 | 21篇 |
2016年 | 26篇 |
2015年 | 26篇 |
2014年 | 32篇 |
2013年 | 71篇 |
2012年 | 32篇 |
2011年 | 28篇 |
2010年 | 19篇 |
2009年 | 25篇 |
2008年 | 36篇 |
2007年 | 23篇 |
2006年 | 24篇 |
2005年 | 17篇 |
2004年 | 27篇 |
2003年 | 15篇 |
2002年 | 16篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 20篇 |
1999年 | 20篇 |
1998年 | 12篇 |
1997年 | 16篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 15篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 12篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 13篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1969年 | 5篇 |
1967年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有812条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
Children's working understanding of knowledge sources: Confidence in knowledge gained from testimony
In three experiments, children aged between 3 and 5 years (N = 38, 52, 94; mean ages 3–7 to 5–2) indicated their confidence in their knowledge of the identity of a hidden toy. With the exception of some 3-year-olds, children revealed working understanding of their knowledge source by showing high confidence when they had seen or felt the toy, and lower confidence when they had been told its identity by an apparently well-informed speaker. Correct explicit source reports were not necessary for children to show relative uncertainty when the speaker subsequently doubted the adequacy of his access to the toy. After a 2-min delay, 3–4-year-olds, unlike 4–5-year-olds, failed to see the implications of the speaker's doubt about his access. 相似文献
92.
Michael D. Robinson Benjamin M. Wilkowski 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2008,44(1):65-79
Four studies involving 230 undergraduates examined the interactive effects of dispositional approach and avoidance, as manifest in the traits of extraversion and neuroticism. Participants who were high in both traits or low in both traits exhibited less assertive knocking behavior (Study 1), had difficulties refraining from blinking (Study 2), and displayed more anxious behavior during a mental health interview (Study 3). Study 4 tested the idea that such extraversion by neuroticism interactions might be associated with difficulties in recognizing and responding to goal-relevant stimuli. Results involving a go/no go task confirmed this hypothesis. In total, the results highlight the manner in which the co-activation of dispositional motives related to approach and avoidance can compromise effective self-regulation. 相似文献
93.
Hurt TR Beach SR Stokes LA Bush PL Sheats KJ Robinson SG 《Cultural diversity & ethnic minority psychology》2012,18(3):312-315
To better meet the needs of eligible African American men who were reluctant to enroll in a 5-year study called the Program for Strong African American Marriages (ProSAAM), we employed two focus groups to listen to the voices of a sample of the population being recruited and to explore their feedback about taking part in such a marriage enrichment program and how best to recruit other African American men. We explain our application of the focus group results to our recruitment of African American married couples for ProSAAM. The positive outcomes from the implementation of these results can provide potential ideas for those seeking innovative means to improve recruitment of African American men to programs that strengthen marital relationships. 相似文献
94.
Michael Robinson 《Philosophical Studies》2012,157(2):177-194
A great deal of attention has been paid recently to the claim that traditional Frankfurt-type counterexamples to the Principle
of Alternative Possibilities (PAP), which depend for their success on the presence of a perfectly reliable indicator (or prior sign) of what an agent will freely do if left to act on his own, are guilty of begging the question against incompatibilists,
since such indicators seem to presuppose a deterministic relation between an agent’s free action and its causal antecedents.
Objections of this sort have given rise to considerable efforts to construct alternative Frankfurt-type counterexamples that
do not rely on prior signs of this kind and so do not presuppose determinism in a way that incompatibilists should find objectionable.
One consequence of this shift in the way Frankfurt-type counterexamples are formulated is that it provides an opportunity
for the forceful resurgence of certain versions of the so-called flicker defense of PAP. In this paper I develop two versions of the flicker defense, indicate their advantages over other versions of this
strategy, and defend them against objections. Insofar as either of these is successful, it will show not only that PAP has
yet to be falsified by any of the modified Frankfurt-type counterexamples currently on offer but that cases of this sort are
in principle incapable of falsifying PAP. 相似文献
95.
Solution concepts for game play implicitly assume what information is relevant for choosing moves. Maximin and Eliminating Dominated Strategies use payoff order; mixed strategies and Harsanyi??s risk dominance use payoff values. ??Cooperative?? strategies use previous choices, ignoring payoffs altogether. We first define the 12 symmetric 2 × 2 games as a continuous payoff space then use this space to evaluate strategies based on different types of information. Strategic success is shown to be sensitive to actual payoffs and seldom consistent for all games of an ordinally defined category such as Chicken. A new hybrid strategy using both payoff and choice information is introduced and shown to produce efficient outcomes in all symmetric 2 × 2 games when employed by both players. It does well in evolutionary tournaments against strategies of other types, especially in the difficult distribution games including Battle-of-the-Sexes. 相似文献
96.
Ryan L. Boyd Michael D. Robinson Adam K. Fetterman 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2011,47(6):1192-1197
Three experiments (total N = 291) sought to provide support for the idea that avoidance conflicts, relative to approach conflicts, are more difficult to motorically resolve. Conflicts were instantiated by asking individuals to approach desirable stimuli and avoid undesirable stimuli under conditions in which there was no objectively-correct direction of movement. To control for baseline movement speeds, non-conflict trials presented desirable (e.g., reward) and undesirable (e.g., threat) stimuli in the absence of any spatial conflict. In addition, movement times were isolated through the use of a joystick movement paradigm in which movement speeds were quantified subsequent to some initial tendency to move in one direction or the other. Consistent with hypotheses, all experiments found that movement times were slowed in the context of avoidance conflicts relative to approach conflicts. Results are discussed in terms of theories of motivation, affective processing, conflict, and anxiety. 相似文献
97.
Espelage Dorothy L. Valido Alberto Robinson Luz E. Ingram Katherine M. El Sheikh America Woolweaver Ashley M. Koritz Laura Vincent Claudia G. Marquez Brion Walker Hill M. Svanks Rita Marmolejos Roslyn Reynoso Medina Courtney Meltsner Zeke Yalamanchi Kovida Pennefather Jordan 《Social Psychology of Education》2022,25(5):1177-1203
Social Psychology of Education - Creating effective school-wide safety mechanisms depends on understanding students' previous experiences of reporting, however, students may also be... 相似文献
98.
Dana H. Z. Robinson Susan M. Gerbensky Klammer Jennie P. Perryman Nancy J. Thompson Kimberly R. Jacob Arriola 《Journal of religion and health》2014,53(6):1857-1872
African Americans are overrepresented on the organ transplant waiting list and underrepresented among organ and tissue donors. One of the most highly noted reasons for lack of donation is the perception that donation is contrary to religious beliefs. The purpose of this cross-sectional study is to explore the complexities of religion (beliefs, religiosity, and religious involvement) and its association with willingness to donate and the written expression of donation intentions. Findings from a sample of 505 African American participants suggest that religion is a multidimensional construct and results differ depending on how the construct is measured and operationalized. 相似文献
99.
Why are some mental tasks experienced as more effortful than others? Answers to this question about subjective effort have begun to be addressed by researchers investigating why some mental tasks are associated with more ‘elbow grease’ or ‘depletion’ than other tasks. It has been proposed that tasks such as sustained attention should be accompanied by more subjective effort than other tasks, such as assessing (e.g., counting a handful of items) or choosing randomly between two alternatives. In general, these proposals coincide with people’s intuitions regarding how effort should vary by mental task. However, little laboratory data have corroborated these conclusions. In two studies, we cataloged the relative amount of subjective effort associated with some basic and ubiquitous mental activities: attending (most subjective effort), assessing, and choosing (least subjective effort). Results support hypotheses about subjective effort. Because subjective effort is perceived to be experienced by a subject, we explored also the relationship between effort and the involvement of the ‘psychological self.’ 相似文献
100.
Michael D. Robinson Konrad Bresin 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2014,36(2):201-210
A review of the literature suggests that higher levels of psychopathy may be linked to less effective behavioral control. However, several commentators have urged caution in making statements of this type in the absence of direct evidence. In two studies (total N?=?142), moment-to-moment accuracy in a motor control task was examined as a function of dimensional variations in psychopathy in an undergraduate population. As hypothesized, motor control was distinctively worse at higher levels of psychopathy relative to lower levels, both as a function of primary and secondary psychopathy and particularly their shared variance. These novel findings provide support for the idea that motor control systematically varies by psychopathy, in a basic manner, consistent with views of psychopathy emphasizing lesser control. 相似文献