全文获取类型
收费全文 | 17938篇 |
免费 | 885篇 |
国内免费 | 15篇 |
专业分类
18838篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 149篇 |
2020年 | 266篇 |
2019年 | 285篇 |
2018年 | 404篇 |
2017年 | 420篇 |
2016年 | 433篇 |
2015年 | 328篇 |
2014年 | 347篇 |
2013年 | 1466篇 |
2012年 | 634篇 |
2011年 | 734篇 |
2010年 | 412篇 |
2009年 | 403篇 |
2008年 | 525篇 |
2007年 | 497篇 |
2006年 | 429篇 |
2005年 | 363篇 |
2004年 | 361篇 |
2003年 | 341篇 |
2002年 | 349篇 |
2001年 | 709篇 |
2000年 | 705篇 |
1999年 | 466篇 |
1998年 | 189篇 |
1997年 | 176篇 |
1996年 | 146篇 |
1995年 | 138篇 |
1993年 | 139篇 |
1992年 | 365篇 |
1991年 | 323篇 |
1990年 | 347篇 |
1989年 | 342篇 |
1988年 | 321篇 |
1987年 | 292篇 |
1986年 | 313篇 |
1985年 | 304篇 |
1984年 | 237篇 |
1983年 | 203篇 |
1979年 | 193篇 |
1978年 | 162篇 |
1975年 | 187篇 |
1974年 | 243篇 |
1973年 | 199篇 |
1972年 | 203篇 |
1971年 | 167篇 |
1970年 | 149篇 |
1969年 | 199篇 |
1968年 | 202篇 |
1967年 | 161篇 |
1966年 | 183篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Samuel L. Odom Lynette K. Chandler Michaelene Ostrosky Scott R. McConnell Susan Reaney 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1992,25(2):307-317
This study examined a system for fading teacher prompts to children who served as peers in peer-initiation interventions for young children with disabilities. A teacher taught peers to direct social initiations to children with disabilities, provided verbal prompts for those initiations, and introduced a system that provided peers with visual feedback about the social interactions of the children with disabilities. She then systematically withdrew the verbal prompts to peers, and subsequently faded the visual feedback system. Peer initiations increased when the intervention began and resulted in increases in social interaction for the children with disabilities. As the teacher systematically faded the prompts and visual feedback to the peers, social interaction continued at the levels found during intervention and was maintained during a short maintenance period. 相似文献
72.
This study tested the effect of using the word stress in the measurement of self-reported occupational stressors and strains. Employees from two organizations responded to a questionnaire that included specific occupational stressors, strains, and 16 items in which the word stress was used. Survey respondents tended to interpret the word stress to refer both to employees' strains or reactions to the work environment and to job stressors or elements of the environment itself. Implications of these findings for occupational stress research are discussed. 相似文献
73.
Anthony J. Cuvo Paula K. Davis Mark F. O'Reilly Brenda M. Mooney Ruth Crowley 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1992,25(2):477-489
We assessed whether written task analyses would serve as textual prompts for performing functional tasks by persons with mild disabilities. Several variables that could influence the effectiveness of textual prompts to promote stimulus control were examined across four groups. A consistent finding was that written specific task analyses combined with end-of-trial performance feedback were effective for promoting the acquisition and generalization of several tasks. Performance transferred immediately to natural discriminative stimuli when the written task analyses and feedback were withdrawn for most tasks and participants. For 2 participants, transfer of stimulus control was accomplished by prompt fading, using individualized written task analyses either with or without performance feedback (Group 1). When feedback was not provided, the effectiveness of written specific task analyses was inconsistent across groups. In contrast to the controlling effects of written specific task analyses, written generic task analyses, which specified only major task outcomes, when combined with performance feedback (Group 1) did not control responding. Overall, this research demonstrated the effectiveness of written specific task analyses and performance feedback to promote stimulus control for persons with mild disabilities. 相似文献
74.
75.
This investigation was conducted to examine whether older adults would improve their performance when given statements intended to make them feel important to a research study as well as statements concerning their over-all well-being. Women (n = 14) and men (n = 15) with mean ages of 76.0 yr. and 75.1 yr., respectively, were randomly assigned into a control group, a positive reinforcement group, and a group given statements of personal importance. Dependent measures on a rotary pursuit task, screw-and-nut assembly task, and an interest questionnaire were obtained during three separate visits. Data were analyzed with a 3 x 3 (Groups x Days) analysis of variance. While no significant group effects were found on any measure, a significant main effect was obtained for days, indicative of learning. 相似文献
76.
30 subjects participated in a discrimination experiment learning face-letter associations under four rotation conditions (45 degrees, 90 degrees, 135 degrees, 180 degrees). Under each condition two thirds of the faces were presented twice, upright and rotated away from the vertical; the remaining faces were presented once, upright or rotated. Learning is described by a joint Markov model: For faces that are presented twice it assumes a separate association and encoding process (two-stage-model), for faces that are presented once it assumes an association process (all-or-none-model). The Markov model fits the data for all four rotation conditions. The angle of rotation does not affect learning for faces that are presented once. For faces that are presented twice it influences both the association and the encoding process. For the angles employed, the effect of rotation can be approximated linearly. The results suggest that the encoding of a rotated face differs increasingly from an upright face as a function of these angles of rotation. This confirms analogous conclusions from mental rotation experiments. 相似文献
77.
A series of experiments investigated whether people could integrate nonspatial information about an object with their knowledge of the object's location in space. In Experiments 1 and 3, subjects learned the locations of cities on a fictitious road map; in Experiments 2, 4, and 5, subjects were already familiar with the locations of buildings on a campus. The subjects then learned facts about the cities on the maps or the buildings on the campus. The question of interest was whether or not these nonspatial facts would be integrated in memory with the spatial knowledge. After learning the facts, subjects were given a location-judgment test in which they had to decide whether an object was in one region of the space or another. Knowledge integration was assessed by comparing levels of performance in two conditions: (a) when a city or a building name was primed by a fact about a neighboring city or building, and (b) when a city or a building name was primed by a fact about a distant city or building. Results showed that responses in Condition a were faster or more accurate, or both faster and more accurate, than responses in Condition b. These results indicate that the spatial and nonspatial information were encoded in a common memory representation. 相似文献
78.
S. Alexander Haslam John C. Turner Penelope J. Oakes Craig McGarty Brett K. Hayes 《European journal of social psychology》1992,22(1):3-20
During the 1990-1991 Persian Gulf conflict an experiment was conducted with Australian university students (N = 200) to investigate whether the social stereotyping of Americans varied with social contextual manipulations related to the hostilities. The study, conducted in two phases at the start and end of the conflict, examined how the assignment of standard stereotypical traits to Americans was affected (a) by the large-scale social change constituted by the war and (b) by variation in the frame of reference provided by relevant comparison groups. The elicited stereotypes were sensitive to both of these contextual variables, demonstrating significant variation and fluidity. Overall, stereotypes of Americans were relatively negative. They were significantly more negative (a) at the end of the war than at the beginning in the restricted frame (when Australia and Britain were comparison groups) and (b) in the first phase of the conflict when the frame was extended to include Iraq as a comparison group. The findings were in line with expectations derived from self-categorization theory (Turner, 1985) that the social categorization of self and other into ingroup and outgroup is inherently variable, comparative and context-dependent. They question the long-held view of stereotypes as fixed, rigid and resistant to change. 相似文献
79.
Influence of animation on dynamical judgments. 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
M K Kaiser D R Proffitt S M Whelan H Hecht 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1992,18(3):669-689
The motions of objects in the environment reflect underlying dynamical constraints and regularities. The conditions under which people are sensitive to natural dynamics are considered. In particular, the article considers what determines whether observers can distinguish canonical and anomalous dynamics when viewing ongoing events. The extent to which such perceptual appreciations are integrated with and influence common-sense reasoning about mechanical events is examined. It is concluded that animation evokes accurate dynamical intuitions when there is only 1 dimension of information that is of dynamical relevance. This advantage is lost when the observed motion reflects higher dimension dynamics or when the kinematic information is removed or degraded. 相似文献
80.
K Rayner R K Morris 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1992,18(1):163-172
Prior experiments reported by Underwood and his colleagues have suggested that information about the informative parts of parafoveal words that have not yet been fixated can influence where readers fixate next. The basic finding that they have reported is that the eyes move farther into a word when the information that uniquely identifies the word is at the end of the word rather than at the beginning of the word. On the basis of such results, it has been suggested that semantic preprocessing influences eye movement behavior in reading. Some theoretical and methodological problems are raised with the prior experiments and then an attempt to replicate the finding is reported. With a highly accurate eyetracking system, the basic finding could not be replicated. An alternative account of eye movement control in reading is discussed. 相似文献