全文获取类型
收费全文 | 17938篇 |
免费 | 885篇 |
国内免费 | 15篇 |
专业分类
18838篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 149篇 |
2020年 | 266篇 |
2019年 | 285篇 |
2018年 | 404篇 |
2017年 | 420篇 |
2016年 | 433篇 |
2015年 | 328篇 |
2014年 | 347篇 |
2013年 | 1466篇 |
2012年 | 634篇 |
2011年 | 734篇 |
2010年 | 412篇 |
2009年 | 403篇 |
2008年 | 525篇 |
2007年 | 497篇 |
2006年 | 429篇 |
2005年 | 363篇 |
2004年 | 361篇 |
2003年 | 341篇 |
2002年 | 349篇 |
2001年 | 709篇 |
2000年 | 705篇 |
1999年 | 466篇 |
1998年 | 189篇 |
1997年 | 176篇 |
1996年 | 146篇 |
1995年 | 138篇 |
1993年 | 139篇 |
1992年 | 365篇 |
1991年 | 323篇 |
1990年 | 347篇 |
1989年 | 342篇 |
1988年 | 321篇 |
1987年 | 292篇 |
1986年 | 313篇 |
1985年 | 304篇 |
1984年 | 237篇 |
1983年 | 203篇 |
1979年 | 193篇 |
1978年 | 162篇 |
1975年 | 187篇 |
1974年 | 243篇 |
1973年 | 199篇 |
1972年 | 203篇 |
1971年 | 167篇 |
1970年 | 149篇 |
1969年 | 199篇 |
1968年 | 202篇 |
1967年 | 161篇 |
1966年 | 183篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
The effects of two alternative sources of food delivery on the key-peck responding of pigeons were examined. Pecking was maintained by a variable-interval 3-min schedule. In the presence of this schedule in different conditions, either a variable-time 3-min schedule delivering food independently of responding or an equivalent schedule that required a minimum 2-s pause between a key peck and food delivery (a differential-reinforcement-of-other-behavior schedule) was added. The differential-reinforcement-of-other-behavior schedule reduced response rates more than did the variable-time schedule in most instances. The delay between a key peck and the next reinforcer consistently was longer under the differential-reinforcement-of-other-behavior schedule than under the variable-time schedule. Response rates and median delay between responses and reinforcers were negatively correlated. These results contradict earlier conclusions about the behavioral effects of alternative reinforcement. They suggest that an interpretation in terms of response–reinforcer contiguity is consistent with the data. 相似文献
42.
K Friemert 《Psychiatrie, Neurologie, und medizinische Psychologie》1988,40(11):671-677
Basing on the author's experiences during his study stay at the Serbsky All-Union Research Institute for General and Forensic Psychiatry in Moscow a report is given about the theoretical foundations and the carrying-out of forensic-psychiatric expert-opinions in the field of penal as well as civil law in the U.S.S.R. Some peculiarities in comparison with the practice in the G.D.R. are taken in special account. 相似文献
43.
S Martin K Housley H Mccoy P Greenhouse F Stigger M A Kenney S Shoffner V Fu M Korslund F G Ercanli-Huffman 《Perceptual and motor skills》1988,67(3):879-884
The Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale was administered to 550 14- and 16-yr.-old (+/- 6 mo.) girls. Self-esteem scores were categorized by weight and weight by height. Scores on the Quetelet Index for obesity were correlated with self-esteem scores. Mean self-esteem of the low- and middle-weight by height group was significantly higher than the mean of the high-weight by height group. In analyzing weight alone, the self-esteem of the middle-weight group was significantly higher than the self-esteem of the high-weight group. The correlation of the obesity index and self-esteem indicated that as weight increased self-esteem decreased. 相似文献
44.
D K Simonton 《Psychological bulletin》1988,104(2):251-267
45.
46.
47.
48.
M H Kelly J K Bock 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1988,14(3):389-403
The goals of this research were to determine whether speakers adjust the stress patterns of words within sentences to create an alternation between strong and weak beats and to explore whether this rhythmic alternation contributes to the characteristics stress differences between two major lexical categories of English. Two experiments suggested that speakers do alter lexical stress in accordance with rhythmic biases. When speakers produced disyllabic pseudowords in sentence contexts, they were more likely to place stress on the first syllable when the pseudoword was preceded by a weak stress and followed by a strong one than when the strong stress preceded and the weak followed. This occurred both when the pseudowords served as nouns and when they served as verbs. Text analyses further revealed that weakly stressed elements precede nouns more often than verbs, whereas such elements follow verbs more often than nouns. Thus, disyllabic nouns are more likely than disyllabic verbs to occupy contexts biased toward trochaic rhythm, a finding consistent with leftward dominant stress in disyllabic English nouns. The history of stress changes in English nouns and verbs also conforms with the view that rhythmic context may have contributed to the evolution of stress differences. Together, the findings suggest that the citation stress patterns of words may to some degree reflect adaptations of lexical knowledge to conditions of language performance. 相似文献
49.
For 3 consecutive days cockroaches (Periplaneta americana) were exposed to either escapable, inescapable, or no shock in an escape task. Twenty-four hours later they were tested in a shuttlebox escape task. There were reliable differences between escapable and inescapable animals and between inescapable and control animals in both escape latencies and the number of failures to escape. 相似文献
50.
Ceci and Liker (1986b) presented data that they contended shows two things: (a) Handicapping harness races is a cognitively complex undertaking that can be captured by a multiple regression model, and (b) neither overall skill at handicapping nor the complexity of the mental model used is related to standard measures of intelligence. The first contention is not at issue. But the second contention, that handicapping performance is unrelated to IQ, is not supported by the data presented; in fact, the opposite conclusion seems more likely. The purpose of this comment is to point out errors frequently made in individual-differences research concerning population definition, sample selection, dependent and independent variable reliability and validity and interpretation of results. 相似文献