全文获取类型
收费全文 | 576篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
579篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 18篇 |
2019年 | 21篇 |
2018年 | 19篇 |
2017年 | 20篇 |
2016年 | 21篇 |
2015年 | 16篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 46篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 21篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 7篇 |
2008年 | 22篇 |
2007年 | 19篇 |
2006年 | 14篇 |
2005年 | 9篇 |
2004年 | 14篇 |
2003年 | 8篇 |
2002年 | 7篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 13篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 12篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 11篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1971年 | 7篇 |
1970年 | 4篇 |
1969年 | 4篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1967年 | 5篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有579条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
When a high-intensity tone (inducer) is followed by a moderate-intensity tone (test tone), the loudness of the latter is reduced. This phenomenon, called induced loudness reduction (ILR), depends on the frequency separation of the two tones; as the difference in frequency increases, the amount of ILR decreases. However, the precise course of this decrease is not well known. This article presents two experiments that address this question. In the first experiment, the amount of loudness reduction produced by a 2.5-kHz 80-dB-SPL inducer was measured with the frequency of the test tone swept from 800 Hz to 6 kHz. In the second experiment, the amount of ILR was measured with the same inducer and with test tones set at 2, 2.5, 3, and 4 kHz. Both experiments show that some ILR occurs at frequency separations as wide as four critical bands. 相似文献
42.
This study examined whether ruminative style moderated the effects of expressive writing. Sixty-nine participants were assessed for ruminative style and depression symptoms at the beginning of their 1st college semester. Participants were then randomized to either an expressive writing or a control writing condition. Changes in depression symptoms were assessed 2, 4, and 6 months later. Results showed that a brooding ruminative style moderated the effects of expressive writing such that among those assigned to the expressive writing condition, individuals with greater brooding scores reported significantly fewer depression symptoms at all of the follow-up assessments relative to individuals with lower brooding scores. In contrast, reflective pondering ruminative style did not moderate the effects of expressive writing on depression symptoms. These findings suggest that expressive writing could be used as a means of reducing depression symptoms among those with a maladaptive ruminative tendency to brood. 相似文献
43.
Mindt MR Byrd D Ryan EL Robbins R Monzones J Arentoft A Germano KK Henniger DE Morgello S 《Cultural diversity & ethnic minority psychology》2008,14(4):315-325
Hispanic individuals in the U.S. have been disproportionately impacted by HIV/AIDS, yet little is known regarding the neuropsychological sequelae of HIV within the Hispanic population. This study characterized neuropsychological (NP) test performance of HIV+ English-speaking Hispanic participants (n = 51) and investigated the combined roles of sociocultural factors (e.g., ethnicity, socioeconomic status [SES] proxy, and reading level) on NP test performance among our HIV+ Hispanic and non-Hispanic White participants (n = 49). Results revealed that the pattern of NP impairment in HIV+ Hispanic participants is consistent with the frontal-striatal pattern observed in HIV-associated CNS sequelae, and the overall prevalence of global NP impairment was high compared to previous reports with more ethnically homogeneous, non-Hispanic White cohorts. Multivariate prediction models that considered both sociocultural factors and CD4 count revealed that reading level was the only unique predictor of global NP functioning, learning, and attention/working memory. In contrast, ethnicity was the only unique predictor of abstraction/executive functioning. This study provides support for the use of neuropsychological evaluation in detecting HIV-associated NP impairment among HIV+ Hispanic participants and adds to the growing literature regarding the importance of considering sociocultural factors in the interpretation of NP test performance. 相似文献
44.
45.
Political Participation and Feminist Consciousness Among Women Activists of the 1960s 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
This paper examined the hypothesis that women who took part in student movements of the 1960s would be distinguishable from their contemporaries in terms of political ideology, political behavior, and feminism in middle age. Women who had been identified as student activists in public records during the late 1960s and early 1970s were compared to a sample of nonactivist peers. Although the two groups scored similarly on variables related to work and family arrangements, former activists scored higher on measures of leftist political orientation and political efficacy, reported greater political salience and collectivism, and reported greater current political participation. Although both groups reported high levels of feminist consciousness and identity, activists scored significantly higher. The difficulty of politically mobilizing women to combat gender discrimination is discussed in light of the discrepancy between consciousness and activism in the comparison group. 相似文献
46.
Smoking in College Women: The Role of Thinness Pressures, Media Exposure, and Critical Consciousness 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Alyssa N. Zucker Zaje A. Harrell Kathie Miner-Rubino Abigail J. Stewart Cynthia S. Pomerleau & Carol J. Boyd 《Psychology of women quarterly》2001,25(3):233-241
There are strong social pressures for U.S. females, particularly those of European heritage, to achieve and maintain an extremely low body weight. These pressures are reflected in a variety of media sources, including advertising. We argue that valuing thinness, exposure to thinness-depicting media, and lacking skepticism about tobacco advertisements have adverse effects on young women's decisions about smoking, particularly smoking for weight control. We tested these hypotheses in a study of 188 female undergraduates, both never-smokers and daily smokers. Believing that smoking controls weight, exposure to thinness-depicting media, and low levels of skepticism about tobacco advertising were associated with being a smoker. Among smokers, believing that smoking controls weight, internalizing thinness pressures, and low levels of feminist consciousness were associated with smoking for weight control. Results are discussed with the aim of encouraging public health anti-smoking campaigns targeted at women, and smoking cessation programs that are responsive to the needs of weight-concerned female smokers. 相似文献
47.
48.
The Ss made judgments of the frontoparallel plane monocularly and binocularly by adjusting the position of a rotatable thin luminous line viewed in darkness. The judgments were made before and after a 10-min period of monocular inspection of a luminous outline frontoparallel trapezoid which was the projection of a rectangle rotated in depth. Verbal reports indicated normalization of perspectival slant depth, i.e., the inspection figure appeared less rotated, and the line settings exhibited an aftereffect bf normalization. These findings may contribute toward an account of certain previously unexplained observations reported in an earlier study by Epstein and Morgan-Paap (1974) dealing with adaptation to uniocular image magnification. 相似文献
49.
Leonard H. Epstein Larry A. Doke Thomas E. Sajwaj Sue Sorrell Betty Rimmer 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1974,7(3):385-390
The effects and side effects of overcorrection for self-stimulatory behaviors of two children in a specialized day-care program were evaluated. For one child, a “hand” overcorrection procedure involving arm and hand exercises was introduced contingent upon inappropriate hand movements and later contingent upon inappropriate foot movements. After “hand” overcorrection was withdrawn for inappropriate foot movements, a “foot” overcorrection procedure involving foot and leg exercises was introduced contingent upon inappropriate foot movements. For a second child, the “hand” overcorrection procedure was introduced contingent upon inappropriate hand movements during a free-play period, and later contingent upon inappropriate vocalizations at naptime. “Hand” overcorrection was withdrawn and then re-introduced sequentially for both behaviors. Several concurrent behaviors were measured to assess multiple effects of treatment. Results for both children indicated the “hand” overcorrection procedure suppressed inappropriate hand movements and inappropriate behaviors that were topographically dissimilar. In addition, inverse relationships were observed between the second child's inappropriate hand movements and appropriate toy usage during free play and between his inappropriate vocalizations and inappropriate foot movements during naptime. Results suggest that overcorrection procedures that are effective for one behavior can be used to reduce the frequency of topographically different behaviors. This finding is discussed in terms of its practical implications for therapists. 相似文献
50.
A recognition memory task was used to determine whether the search set is reduced by cueing subject to forget a part of the input. Correct recognition reaction time was used to infer the size of the search set. The principal results were that RT on forget trials was faster than RT on remember trials and that RT was fastest when the forget cue was presented at the outset of the trial rather than after the material had been presented. These findings are generally consistent with the hypothesis that selective search is responsible for the enhancement of recall produced by forget instructions in earlier studies. 相似文献