全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1459篇 |
免费 | 42篇 |
专业分类
1501篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 19篇 |
2020年 | 24篇 |
2019年 | 20篇 |
2018年 | 33篇 |
2017年 | 56篇 |
2016年 | 53篇 |
2015年 | 31篇 |
2014年 | 48篇 |
2013年 | 152篇 |
2012年 | 63篇 |
2011年 | 68篇 |
2010年 | 30篇 |
2009年 | 27篇 |
2008年 | 55篇 |
2007年 | 48篇 |
2006年 | 36篇 |
2005年 | 48篇 |
2004年 | 41篇 |
2003年 | 25篇 |
2002年 | 53篇 |
2001年 | 25篇 |
2000年 | 30篇 |
1999年 | 22篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 15篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 13篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 20篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 16篇 |
1987年 | 14篇 |
1985年 | 21篇 |
1984年 | 23篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1981年 | 14篇 |
1980年 | 16篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 20篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
1971年 | 12篇 |
1970年 | 12篇 |
1968年 | 18篇 |
1967年 | 16篇 |
排序方式: 共有1501条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
211.
212.
213.
The Fechner wheel is a disk that is printed in black and white. When the Fechner wheel spins at an appropriate speed, semicircles
on the wheel appear to be in color. Modern computer technology makes it possible to rapidly and repeatedly present a series
of images on a monitor screen. A series of still images of a Fechner wheel can be substituted for a Fechner wheel on a motor
shaft. Straight lines and lines of text can be substituted for semicircles to obtain a color effect. A color effect can be
obtained in small areas of black-and-white pictures. 相似文献
214.
This study investigated the relationships of visuomotor constructs as measured by the Bender-Gestalt II with the subtests of the WISC-III. A factor analysis included the standardized Copy and Recall scores of the Bender-Gestalt II and subtest scores of the WISC-III. The Copy score was predicted to load on measures of visual and spatial thinking and the Recall score on a short-term memory factor. The results of a principal components analysis suggest a four-factor solution with the Bender-Gestalt II Copy score loading on a visual and spatial thinking factor primarily with WISC-III Perceptual Organization subtests. The Recall score loaded on a visual and spatial thinking factor as well as a short-term memory factor with the WISC-III Digit Span subtest. The results suggest the Bender-Gestalt II Copy subtest shared commonality with the visual and spatial tasks given the similarity in the visuomotor demands of each test and was less influenced by processing speed or cultural knowledge than other tests on the visual and spatial thinking factor. Also, results suggest Bender-Gestalt II Recall measures elements of both visual and spatial thinking as well as aspects of short-term memory and memory retrieval. Clinical guidelines for co-administration and underlying processing demands are discussed. 相似文献
215.
40 college males were sequentially assigned to 1 of 2 groups to examine the effects of induced physical fatigue upon the performance and learning of a gross motor task, the stabilometer. All Ss were given 32 practice trials over 3 practice sessions, with 48 hr. rest interpolated between sessions. Trials 1 and 2 were performed under control conditions (no fatigue) for both groups. The Experimental Group was then required to perform under conditions of physical fatigue during Trials 3-26. Trials 27-32 (Session 3) were performed under control conditions. The condition of fatigue was achieved on Trials 3-26 by having Ss pedal a bicycle ergometer until a heart rate of 180 beats/min was attained prior to each trial. The Control Group cancelled vowels. The results indicated that physical fatigue was detrimental to the performance and learning of the Experimental Group. 相似文献
216.
Ric G. Steele Jason Van Allen Eric R. Benson Heather L. Hunter Diane McDermott 《Journal of personality assessment》2013,95(4):375-381
The investigators factor-analyzed abstract art preferences and correlated art factors with traits on the Guilford-Zimmerman Temperament Survey. Factors were derived from preference ratings on the “Abstract Art Test” developed by Knapp and Green in a previous study. Two of the three multiple loading factors were “non-geometric” or expressionistic; one was “geometric,” rationalistic. Only 2 of the 30 correlations with the GZTS scales were significant at the .10 level, and these could have been due to chance. There is support for dichotomizing abstract art types into “geometric” and “non-geometric” but little support for the role of temperament in aesthetic preference. 相似文献
217.
Organizational display rules (e.g., “service with a smile”) have had mixed relationships with employee emotional labor—either in the form of “bad faith” surface acting (suppressing or faking expressions) or “good faith” deep acting (modifying inner feelings). We draw on the motivational perspective of emotional labor to argue that individual differences in customer orientation will directly and indirectly relate to these acting strategies in response to display rules. With a survey of more than 500 working adults in customer contact positions, and controlling for affective disposition, we find that customer orientation directly increases “good faith” acting while it moderates the relationship of display rules with “bad faith” acting. 相似文献
218.
Michael J. Hove John R. Iversen Allen Zhang Bruno H. Repp 《Psychological research》2013,77(4):388-398
Synchronization of finger taps with periodically flashing visual stimuli is known to be much more variable than synchronization with an auditory metronome. When one of these rhythms is the synchronization target and the other serves as a distracter at various temporal offsets, strong auditory dominance is observed. However, it has recently been shown that visuomotor synchronization improves substantially with moving stimuli such as a continuously bouncing ball. The present study pitted a bouncing ball against an auditory metronome in a target–distracter synchronization paradigm, with the participants being auditory experts (musicians) and visual experts (video gamers and ball players). Synchronization was still less variable with auditory than with visual target stimuli in both groups. For musicians, auditory stimuli tended to be more distracting than visual stimuli, whereas the opposite was the case for the visual experts. Overall, there was no main effect of distracter modality. Thus, a distracting spatiotemporal visual rhythm can be as effective as a distracting auditory rhythm in its capacity to perturb synchronous movement, but its effectiveness also depends on modality-specific expertise. 相似文献
219.
Robert M. Kaplan PH.D J. Allen McCutchan M.D Ana M. Navarro PH.D. John P. Anderson PH.D. J. Hampton Atkinson M.D. James Chandler M.D. 《Psychology & health》2013,28(1-2):131-141
Abstract Background In survival analysis, those who are alive are statistically coded as 1.0 while those who are dead are coded as 0.0. Since everyone who remains alive is given the same score, a person confined to bed with an irreversible coma is alive and is counted the same as someone who is active and asymtomatic. The Quality of Well-being (QWB) scale defines levels of wellness on the continuum between death and optimum function and integrates morbidity and mortality into the same number. This paper demonstrates the effect of including mortality in QWB estimates for male adults with HIV infection. Method This study involves follow-up of a cohort of 386 male adults participating in the San Diego HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center (HNRC). Patients were evaluated using the QWB at enrollment and at six month intervals. All patients were classified into three stages of HIV disease according to the Center for Disease Control (CDC) classification: CDC IV (symptomatic HIV disease), CDC II or III (asymptomatic infection) and uninfected male controls. Results QWB scores were calculated with and without mortality included for men in each CDC class who completed a one year (N = 148) or two year follow-up (N = 60). At each evaluation, there were significant differences among CDC classes and inclusion of deaths increased the variance accounted for by CDC class at each evaluation. Conclusions HIV infection has significant impacts upon both morbidity and mortality. Survival analysis captures only the mortality dimension, while quality adjusted survival analysis using the QWB includes both dimensions. We propose quality adjusted survival analysis as a more sensitive method for assessing outcome in HIV disease and other health conditions. 相似文献
220.
Bronna D. Romanoff Allen G. Israel George C. Tremblay Michael R. O'neill Helena A. Roderick 《Journal of Loss and Trauma》2013,18(4):293-308
Abstract Losses are often equated for conceptual purposes, and it is often assumed that different types of losses result in similar grief reactions and exert similar effects on cognitive structures and coping patterns. This article examines the expectancies, beliefs, coping patterns, and psychological adjustment associated with loss due to death, parental. divorce, or illness and disability in a college student population. Loss groups did not differ from each other or from a no-loss group on cognitive variables, coping, or psychological adjustment. In instances in which associations between the cognitive measures and either coping or adjustment were indicated, the typical pattern was for loss groups to differ from the no-loss condition but not from each other. 相似文献