全文获取类型
收费全文 | 604篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
616篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 5篇 |
2019年 | 8篇 |
2018年 | 9篇 |
2017年 | 10篇 |
2016年 | 16篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 18篇 |
2013年 | 65篇 |
2012年 | 16篇 |
2011年 | 27篇 |
2010年 | 14篇 |
2009年 | 12篇 |
2008年 | 17篇 |
2007年 | 22篇 |
2006年 | 18篇 |
2005年 | 17篇 |
2004年 | 20篇 |
2003年 | 20篇 |
2002年 | 17篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 11篇 |
1978年 | 9篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 12篇 |
1974年 | 12篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
1970年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 5篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有616条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
381.
During adaptation, two different letter strings (each five or six letters) were presented to subjects alternately, one in green and the other in magenta. The extent to which these letter strings subsequently elicited a color aftereffect was assessed. In different experiments, the chromatic letter strings consisted of words and nonwords. The results indicated that letter strings that form English words can contingently elicit a color aftereffect. This was the case even when the words were anagrams. There was no evidence that nonword letter strings could contingently elicit such an aftereffect, even when the nonwords conformed to English orthography. The results are relevant to understanding other contingent color aftereffects (McCollough effects), illusory color noted by computer operators who work at monochrome (green or amber) displays, and the processing of text. 相似文献
382.
Self-Concepts, Domain Values, and Self-Esteem: Relations and Changes at Early Adolescence 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Jacquelynne S. Eccles Allan Wigfield Constance A. Flanagan Christy Miller David A. Reuman Doris Yee 《Journal of personality》1989,57(2):283-310
We assessed how children's self-concepts of ability for mathematics, English, social, and physical skills activities, ratings of the importance of these activities, and general self-esteem change across the transition to junior high school. Three types of change were assessed: change in mean levels, change in stability, and change in relationships. Twice each year during the sixth and seventh grades, 1,450 children completed questionnaires. Mean levels of children's self-esteem were lowest immediately after the transition, but recovered during seventh grade. Self-concept of ability and importance ratings for math and sports activities showed linear declines. Self-concept of ability for social activities showed a cubic trend, but importance ratings for social activities declined in a linear fashion. Children's self-concepts of ability for math and English became less stable across the junior high transition, whereas beliefs about other activities and general self-esteem were more stable in seventh grade. 相似文献
383.
Allan Arkush 《Jewish History》2006,20(3-4):377-379
384.
Jason M. Tangen Lorraine G. Allan 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology》2003,56(3):279-300
It is well established that two predictor cues ( A and B ) of a common outcome interact in that the judgement of the relationship between each cue and the outcome is influenced by the pairing history of the other cue with that outcome. For example, when the contingency of A with an outcome is weaker than the contingency of B with that outcome, the rating of the predictiveness of A is reduced relative to a situation where only A is paired with the outcome. One explanation of such cue interaction effects is provided by the conditional ΔP account. Spellman (1996b) derived a counterintuitive prediction of the conditional ΔP account where cue interaction should not occur under certain conditions even though a relatively poor predictor of an outcome is paired with a relatively good predictor of that outcome. However, Spellman (1996b) did not provide data to evaluate this prediction. In the present paper, we report the relevant data and show that they are consistent with the conditional ΔP account. A competing account of cue interaction is provided by the Rescorla-Wagner (RW) model. We derive the predictions of the RW model for the conditions specified by Spellman (1996b), and show that at asymptote the predictions of the RW model are identical to those of the conditional ΔP account. 相似文献
385.
A study is described that was carried out to determine the feasibility of making interactive graphics terminals available for computer-aided instruction (CAI) in engineering and science courses. 相似文献
386.
B. Dennis Sales Ralph Norman Haber R. Allan Cole 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1968,4(6):321-327
To determine the processing of vowel sounds in short-term memory for a serial recall task, 100 Ss heard either a short string of isolated vowel sounds, or a string in which each of these same sounds was embedded between the consonants “h” and “d”. In contrast to findings by Wickelgren, neither an articulatory or an acoustic distinctive-feature analysis predicted the pattern of intrusion errors found. The overall recall of the different sounds was predicted by the ease with which they could be labelled for rehearsal. However, ease of labelling would not explain the pattern of intrusion errors, nor would any other analysis tried. These results are consistent with a coding model presented by Liberman et al (J967). Surprisingly, the patterns of intrusion errors were very similar whether the sounds were presented alone or embedded in words. The implications of these findings for distinctive feature theory and the encoding process are discussed. 相似文献
387.
Male melancholia, rooted in early childhood experiences of perceived mother-loss, is intrinsically linked to religiosity. The Protestant Reformer Martin Luther suffered from melancholia that was related to, and exacerbated by, a corresponding obsessive-compulsive disorder. This essay makes a case for Luther's melancholia being grounded in both childhood beatings (at least one of which was carried out by his mother) and his subsequent search for an identity. Luther's melancholia also gave rise to life-long struggles with obsessive-compulsive anxieties. His religion, in which he believed he had discovered both an identity and a means for relief from his inner struggles, actually exacerbated his melancholia. He realized as an older man that religion had indeed become his substitute obsession and that a major part of his self had died. An argument for how Luther's melancholia and obsessive-compulsive disorder could have been alleviated is offered. 相似文献
388.
Allan M. Schrier Claudia R. Thompson 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1980,33(2):291-298
Carter and Werner recently reviewed the literature on conditional discrimination learning by pigeons, which consists of studies of matching-to-sample and oddity-from-sample. They also discussed three models of such learning: the “multiple-rule” model (learning of stimulus-specific relations), the “configuration” model, and the “single-rule” model (concept learning). Although their treatment of the multiple-rule model, which seems most applicable to the pigeon data, is generally excellent, their discussion of the other two models is incomplete and sometimes inaccurate. Potential problems of terminology are discussed in the present paper, as are additional lines of research that deserve consideration by those interested in further work in this area. The issue of response versus stimulus selection (configuration versus compound-cue learning) is discussed in connection with the configuration model. Particular attention is given to Carter and Werner's criticism of the application, in studies with other species, of the learning set procedure in testing for single-rule learning. Some of the important related issues are: the bias for improvement on new problems in a series, the adequacy of a multiple-rule model to explain learning set formation, and evidence in favor of the single-rule model, at least in primates. Consideration of these additional contributions to the study of conditional discrimination learning emphasizes the usefulness of this task in the comparative study of cognitive processes. 相似文献
389.
The sensitivity of pigeons' schedule-induced activity to operant consequences was studied in two experiments. During a 30-s interval between food presentations, a keylight stimulus brightened incrementally. Stable terminal key pecking and interim locomotor activity developed. An operant "setback" contingency was applied to activity. The contingency arranged for locomotor movements (detected by a nine-panel floorboard) to be followed by a resetting of the keylight brightness to a dimmer value and a 1-s delay of reinforcement (for individual responses). Experiment 1 showed that activity patterns were highly sensitive to their operant consequences. Accompanying key-peck rates were only transiently affected. In Experiment 2, the setback contingency was imposed during restricted portions of the trial, and differential operant control of activity was demonstrated. However, birds in this study produced higher rates of key pecking as activity rates were reduced. These results suggest that although schedule-induced activity arises in response to the temporal arrangement of stimulus events, this behavior may retain considerable sensitivity to response-consequence relations. 相似文献
390.
Seth C Kalichman Demetria Cain Lance Weinhardt Eric Benotsch Kelly Presser Allan Zweben Bonnie Bjodstrup Geoffrey R Swain 《Health psychology》2005,24(2):198-208
An experimental components analysis of brief HIV risk-reduction counseling based on the information-motivation-behavioral skills (IMB) model was conducted with 432 men and 193 women receiving sexually transmitted infection (STI) clinic services. Following baseline assessments, participants were randomly assigned to 1 of 4 90-min risk-reduction counseling sessions that deconstructed the IMB model within a full factorial design. Participants were followed for 9 months, with STI diagnoses monitored over 12 months. Men who received the full IMB session evidenced relatively greater use of risk-reduction behavioral skills and relatively lower rates of unprotected intercourse over 6-months follow-up and had fewer new STIs. For women, however, the motivational counseling demonstrated the most positive outcomes. Results suggest that brief single-exposure HIV prevention counseling can reduce HIV transmission risks. 相似文献