全文获取类型
收费全文 | 151篇 |
免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
156篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 1篇 |
2020年 | 1篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 3篇 |
2017年 | 7篇 |
2016年 | 4篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 8篇 |
2013年 | 25篇 |
2012年 | 10篇 |
2011年 | 15篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 4篇 |
2008年 | 6篇 |
2007年 | 4篇 |
2006年 | 4篇 |
2005年 | 9篇 |
2004年 | 2篇 |
2003年 | 3篇 |
2002年 | 5篇 |
2001年 | 1篇 |
2000年 | 1篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有156条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Elizabeth Aveleyra Sebastin Carranza‐Lira Alfredo Ulloa‐Aguirre Feggy Ostrosky‐Solís 《International journal of psychology》2005,40(5):314-323
Recent reports suggest that hormone therapy (HT) with estrogen may have a protective effect on the ageing brain and cognitive function. However, clinical evidence regarding the cognitive effects in menopausal women under HT has produced conflicting results. The purpose of the present study was to assess and compare the cognitive effects after 6 months of HT in 30 early postmenopausal women, who were divided into three groups as follows: group I, Therapy conjugated equine estrogen (ET) CEE 0.625 mg/day (n = 10); group II, Estrogen‐Progestin Therapy (EPT), CEE 0.625 mg/day plus, chlormadinone 1 mg/day (n = 10); and group III, the control group, who did not receive treatment (n = 10). The three groups were matched by age and years of education. Exclusion criteria were: central nervous system diseases, severe cardiac disease, and clinical history of cancer and depression. Subjects were tested using a comprehensive battery for the evaluation of attention, memory and executive functions, which was standardized and validated in Spanish‐speaking subjects. The rate of cognitive change was defined by the difference between the measurements at the sixth month minus the baseline score. Mean group differences were assessed with MANOVA, followed by one‐way ANOVA considering statistical significance when p < .05; the alpha significance level .05 was corrected using the Bonferroni procedure. The EPT group showed higher scores than the control group and ET group in the Total Attention Score and in the copy of the Rey‐Osterreith Complex Figure. The ET group showed significantly higher scores than the control group and the EPT group in the subtest of Spatial Backward Span and in the Immediate Face Codification. The short‐term positive effects observed with HT in this sample could be related to the stimulation of brain receptors and/or neurotrophic factors that are still present at this age. 相似文献
72.
73.
A head camera was used to examine the visual correlates of object name learning by toddlers as they played with novel objects and as the parent spontaneously named those objects. The toddlers’ learning of the object names was tested after play, and the visual properties of the head camera images during naming events associated with learned and unlearned object names were analyzed. Naming events associated with learning had a clear visual signature, one in which the visual information itself was clean and visual competition among objects was minimized. Moreover, for learned object names, the visual advantage of the named target over competitors was sustained, both before and after the heard name. The findings are discussed in terms of the visual and cognitive processes that may depend on clean sensory input for learning and also on the sensory–motor, cognitive, and social processes that may create these optimal visual moments for learning. 相似文献
74.
The Social Context of Organized Nonbelief: County‐Level Predictors of Nonbeliever Organizations in the United States 下载免费PDF全文
Many recent social scientific studies have noted that the percentage of Americans with no religious affiliation is on the rise, but few have examined the nonbeliever organizations that some of these “nones” might join. This study uses an original data set, the first attempt at documenting the population of local nonbeliever organizations in the United States, to explore where these groups are more likely to flourish. Though one might assume that less religious counties, as measured by the percentage of those with no stated religious affiliation, would be more likely to contain nonbeliever organizations, this article provides evidence that they emerge more frequently and in greater numbers in counties with proportionally more evangelical Protestants. The percentage of evangelicals among a county's population is strongly associated with both the existence (dichotomously coded) and the number of nonbeliever organizations, even when controlling for a range of demographic and institutional factors. 相似文献
75.
Self‐Regulatory Growth Effects for Young Children Participating in a Combined Social and Emotional Learning and Mindfulness‐Based Intervention 下载免费PDF全文
Matthew E. Lemberger‐Truelove Kira J. Carbonneau David J. Atencio Almut K. Zieher Alfredo F. Palacios 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2018,96(3):289-302
The authors analyzed findings from a counselor‐delivered social and emotional learning and mindfulness‐based intervention with twenty‐three 3‐ and 4‐year‐olds from economically disadvantaged backgrounds. Using a multilevel modeling approach to illustrate students' growth across multiple behavioral observations in a randomized controlled design, the authors found that the treatment group exhibited more self‐regulatory‐related behaviors on days when the intervention occurred. Qualitative analyses revealed that the students adopted kindness language consistent with the intervention. Implications for practice are suggested. 相似文献
76.
Alfredo Romagosa 《Cross currents》2020,70(4):378-393
77.
Alfredo Marcos 《Axiomathes》2018,28(6):653-664
The present article offers an introductory vision to the political philosophy of science. The political philosophy of science is a new field of study where the philosophy of science and political philosophy converge. We will see the main contents of this field. We will also note that it depends on the construction of a model of rationality where science and politics can meet each other. Finally, the article tries to outline such a model of rationality. In order to do so, we will review the relationship between Karl Popper’s scientific and political philosophy. I suggest to read Popper’s critical rationalism in terms of a kind of prudential rationality. 相似文献
78.
79.
Dr. Alfredo Ardila Patricia Montañes Consuelo Caro Ricardo Delgado Hugh W. Buckingham 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》1989,18(2):163-180
Thirty-seven aphasic patients whose native language is Spanish were divided into four groups: (1) Broca's, (2) Wernicke's, (3) Conduction, and (4) Anomia. Phonological errors produced on repetition and object-naming tasks were analyzed in terms of type of transformation and degree of similarity between target and errors in terms of shared distinctive features. Segmental transformations were of the following types: (1) pure substitution, (2) pure addition, (3) deletion, (4) reduplicative addition (doublet creation)—anticipatory (right to left) or perseverative (left to right), (5) reduplicative substitution (doublet creation)—anticipatory or perseverative, and (6) mutual exchanges. Pure substitutions were the most prevalent, exchanges, the least. Some error types were found to differentiate significantly between aphasic populations. We suggest that different mechanisms may underlie the same error type for different aphasic populations. 相似文献
80.
Leticia Guarino Derek Roger Daniel Thor Olason 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2007,26(1):37-45
Neuroticism (N) assesses emotional sensitivity or lability, but the construct is poorly defined and instruments used to assess
N are marred by a number of psychometric shortcomings. Neuroticism is also described in an overtly pejorative way, with items
keyed for themes reflecting low self-esteem, anxiety, and depression. The present paper presents a revised model based on
a new questionnaire entitled the Emotional Sensitivity Scale (ESS). Using an innovative scenario technique to generate items,
exploratory factor analysis of the responses of 242 participants yielded orthogonal dimensions for positive (other-oriented)
and negative (self-centered) emotional sensitivity, and the structure was confirmed using confirmatory factor analysis. Validation
studies showed that the ESS dimensions were related in systematic ways to related psychometric instruments and that, as predicted,
participants scoring high on the positive dimension were better able to recognize displayed emotions. The new scale offers
a basis for extending the research on emotional sensitivity using empirically discriminable positive and negative components. 相似文献