首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   165040篇
  免费   7572篇
  国内免费   155篇
  2021年   1484篇
  2020年   2772篇
  2019年   3426篇
  2018年   3415篇
  2017年   3846篇
  2016年   4563篇
  2015年   3903篇
  2014年   4792篇
  2013年   23381篇
  2012年   4291篇
  2011年   3309篇
  2010年   3768篇
  2009年   4695篇
  2008年   3563篇
  2007年   3119篇
  2006年   3795篇
  2005年   3771篇
  2004年   3299篇
  2003年   2945篇
  2002年   2711篇
  2001年   2870篇
  2000年   2772篇
  1999年   2879篇
  1998年   2796篇
  1997年   2620篇
  1996年   2555篇
  1995年   2388篇
  1994年   2359篇
  1993年   2304篇
  1992年   2369篇
  1991年   2254篇
  1990年   2088篇
  1989年   2027篇
  1988年   2004篇
  1987年   2025篇
  1986年   2010篇
  1985年   2230篇
  1984年   2379篇
  1983年   2205篇
  1982年   2330篇
  1981年   2298篇
  1980年   2153篇
  1979年   2050篇
  1978年   2108篇
  1977年   2083篇
  1976年   1866篇
  1975年   1883篇
  1974年   1925篇
  1973年   1796篇
  1972年   1409篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
The authors used recursive partitioning methods to identify combinations of baseline characteristics that predict 2-year physical activity success in each of 3 physical activity interventions delivered in the multisite Activity Counseling Trial. The sample consisted of 874 initially sedentary primary care patients, ages 35-75 years, who were at risk for cardiovascular disease. Predictors of 2-year success were specific to each intervention and represented a range of domains, including physiological, demographic, psychosocial, health-related, and environmental variables. The results indicate how specific patient subgroups (e.g., obese, unfit individuals; high-income individuals in stable health) may respond differently to varying levels and amounts of professional assistance and support. The methods used provide a practical first step toward identifying clinically meaningful patient subgroups for further systematic investigation.  相似文献   
172.
173.
174.
175.
176.
Two experiments examined repetition priming in the recognition of famous voices. In Experiment 1, reaction times for fame decisions to famous voice samples were shorter than in an unprimed condition, when voices were primed by a different voice sample of the same person having been presented in an earlier phase of the experiment. No effect of voice repetition was observed for non-famous voices. In Experiment 2, it was investigated whether this priming effect is voice-specific or whether it is related to post-perceptual processes in person recognition. Recognizing a famous voice was again primed by having earlier heard a different voice sample of that person. Although an earlier exposure to that person's name did not cause any priming, there was some indication of priming following an earlier exposure to that person's face. Finally, earlier exposure to the identical voice sample (as compared to a different voice sample from the same person) caused a considerable bias towards responding 'famous'-i.e. performance benefits for famous but costs for nonfamous voices. The findings suggestthat (1) repetition priming invoice recognition primarily involves the activation of perceptual representations of voices, and (2) it is important to determine the conditions in which priming causes bias effects that need to be disentangled from performance benefits.  相似文献   
177.
178.
179.
To understand how individuals adapt to and anticipate each other in joint tasks, we employ a bidirectional delay–coupled dynamical system that allows for mutual adaptation and anticipation. In delay–coupled systems, anticipation is achieved when one system compares its own time‐delayed behavior, which implicitly includes past information about the other system’s behavior, with the other system’s instantaneous behavior. Applied to joint music performance, the model allows each system to adapt its behavior to the dynamics of the other. Model predictions of asynchrony between two simultaneously produced musical voices were compared with duet pianists’ behavior; each partner performed one voice while auditory feedback perturbations occurred at unpredictable times during live performance. As the model predicted, when auditory feedback from one musical voice was removed, the asynchrony changed: The pianist’s voice that was removed anticipated (preceded) the actions of their partner. When the auditory feedback returned and both musicians could hear each other, they rapidly returned to baseline levels of asynchrony. To understand how the pianists anticipated each other, their performances were fitted by the model to examine change in model parameters (coupling strength, time‐delay). When auditory feedback for one or both voices was removed, the fits showed the expected decrease in coupling strength and time‐delay between the systems. When feedback about the voice(s) returned, the coupling strength and time‐delay returned to baseline. These findings support the idea that when people perform actions together, they do so as a coupled bidirectional anticipatory system.  相似文献   
180.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号