全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2363篇 |
免费 | 50篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
2414篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 24篇 |
2023年 | 30篇 |
2022年 | 23篇 |
2021年 | 54篇 |
2020年 | 73篇 |
2019年 | 77篇 |
2018年 | 82篇 |
2017年 | 104篇 |
2016年 | 97篇 |
2015年 | 53篇 |
2014年 | 79篇 |
2013年 | 271篇 |
2012年 | 105篇 |
2011年 | 110篇 |
2010年 | 75篇 |
2009年 | 69篇 |
2008年 | 97篇 |
2007年 | 88篇 |
2006年 | 71篇 |
2005年 | 74篇 |
2004年 | 44篇 |
2003年 | 50篇 |
2002年 | 52篇 |
2001年 | 28篇 |
2000年 | 38篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 29篇 |
1997年 | 13篇 |
1996年 | 19篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 20篇 |
1993年 | 11篇 |
1992年 | 23篇 |
1991年 | 18篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 20篇 |
1988年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 24篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 15篇 |
1979年 | 21篇 |
1978年 | 14篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 16篇 |
1974年 | 12篇 |
1973年 | 12篇 |
1968年 | 10篇 |
排序方式: 共有2414条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
Tatjana von Solodkoff 《Philosophical Studies》2012,161(3):391-401
In recent work, Louis deRosset (Philosophical Studies 149:73?C97, 2010) has argued that priority theorists, who hold that truths about macroscopic objects can be metaphysically explained without reference to such things, cannot meet an independently motivated constraint upon good explanation. By clarifying the nature of the priority theorist??s project, I argue that deRosset??s argument fails to establish its conclusion. 相似文献
42.
Jeremy P. Nahum Nadia Bruschweiler-Stern Alexandra M. Harrison Karlen Lyons-Ruth Alexander C. Morgan Louis Sander Daniel N. Stern Edward Z. Tronick 《Infant mental health journal》1998,19(3):315-319
A sequence of analytic sessions of a professional woman in her forties is used to illustrate the moving along process. The material shows a gradual change, where the patient's experience of her sense of agency is altered through a particular kind of interactive sequence. The example is intended to make it clear that the group's view of how change occurs is not strictly or primarily in terms of sudden changes, and that the problem of assessing moments of meeting as well as quantitative and qualitative shifts is one with which the group continues to struggle. © 1998 Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health 相似文献
43.
S. Alexander Haslam Penelope J. Oakes Katherine J. Reynolds & Justin Mein 《Asian Journal of Social Psychology》1999,2(2):265-280
This paper examines the impact of social division associated with the emergent "race debate" in Australia (and the so-called "Hanson phenomenon") on the consensus of Australian students' stereotypes of their national ingroup. It compares the stereotypes held by a sample of students in 1997 ( N = 20) with those revealed in studies conducted from 1992 onwards ( N = 102). Results provide strong evidence that stereotype consensus was reduced in the current phase of research and post-testing also indicates that participants themselves interpreted this as being the product of political change in Australian society. The findings are consistent with the argument that stereotypes are sensitive to changes in intergroup and intragroup relations, and that they respond to the reality of social division rather than to the rhetoric of unity. 相似文献
44.
T W Smith J D Sanders J F Alexander 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1990,58(4):699-708
The Cook and Medley (1954) Hostility (Ho) scale has been used in several important studies evaluating potential health consequences of hostility. A relative lack of compelling information about the construct validity of the Ho scale, however, has raised concerns about the appropriate interpretation of previous research. In this study, 60 married couples discussed a low conflict topic, a high conflict topic, and then a second low conflict topic. High Ho men responded to the high conflict topic with significant increases in self-reported anger and anxiety and overt hostile behavior, but low Ho men did not. Furthermore, compared to low Ho men, high Ho men blamed their wives more for their usual disagreements on the high conflict topic and saw their disagreement-engendering behavior as more intentional. Among women, Ho scores were weakly related only to overt hostile behavior. Finally, couples consisting of two low Ho persons displayed a uniquely agreeable interactional style. 相似文献
45.
Summary Five experiments were conducted to determine how distortion of spatial position induced by unidirectional Müller-Lyer fins varied as a function of angle and length of fins. Research employing Cornsweet's staircase method yielded ambiguous results, but psychophysical methods of magnitude estimation, paired comparisons, and production showed conclusively that distortions of position are affected by angle and length of fins in a manner similar to that found in distortions of length. It was concluded that similar strategies are employed in processing attributes of length and position and that a theory based on averaging of attributes within an attentional field describes the performance of real observers.Experiments 1 and 2 were reported in a Master's thesis submitted by Nancy Smith to the University of Manitoba in 1987. 相似文献
46.
Causal reasoning is crucial to people’s decision making in probabilistic environments. It may rely directly on data about covariation between variables (correspondence) or on inferences based on reasonable constraints if larger causal models are constructed based on local relations (coherence). For causal chains an often assumed constraint is transitivity. For probabilistic causal relations, mismatches between such transitive inferences and direct empirical evidence may lead to distortions of empirical evidence. Previous work has shown that people may use the generative local causal relations A → B and B → C to infer a positive indirect relation between events A and C, despite data showing that these events are actually independent (von Sydow et al. in Proceedings of the thirty-first annual conference of the cognitive science society. Cognitive Science Society, Austin, 2009, Proceedings of the 32nd annual conference of the cognitive science society. Cognitive Science Society, Austin, 2010, Mem Cogn 44(3):469–487, 2016). Here we used a sequential learning scenario to investigate how transitive reasoning in intransitive situations with negatively related distal events may relate to betting behavior. In three experiments participants bet as if they were influenced by a transitivity assumption, even when the data strongly contradicted transitivity. 相似文献
47.
Anna Andreas Yvonne Otto Stephanie Stadelmann Andrea Schlesier-Michel Kai von Klitzing Annette M. Klein 《Journal of child and family studies》2017,26(1):148-160
Our longitudinal study contributes to the body of literature on depression in childhood by examining potential protective factors. We investigated the influence of maternal depressive symptoms on child depressive symptoms at early school age and its gender-specific moderation by children’s narrative representations of the maternal figure, over and above children’s own depressive symptoms at preschool age. Children’s narrative representations were assessed using the MacArthur Story Stem Battery in 170 preschool children (92 girls; 54.1?%) oversampled for internalizing symptoms. Children’s depressive symptoms were assessed at preschool age (Time 1; 4–6 years) and at early school age (Time 2; 6–8 years) by maternal report; mothers’ depressive symptoms were assessed at Time 1. The results showed that for boys, only their own depressive symptoms at Time 1 predicted their depressive symptoms at Time 2. For girls, maternal depressive symptoms were a significant risk factor for their own depressive symptoms at Time 2. Regarding this association, we also found a moderation effect: girls with more positive narrative representations of the maternal figure showed a reduced negative impact of maternal depressive symptoms on their own depressive symptoms at Time 2. This implies that clinical practice should screen a child for early depressive symptoms, especially if the mother displays depressive symptoms. Clinicians might also assess children’s inner working models, which can serve as a protective factor. 相似文献
48.
Alexander Reutlinger 《Journal for General Philosophy of Science》2017,48(1):143-150
Renormalization group (RG) explanations account for the astonishing phenomenon that microscopically very different physical systems display the same macro-behavior when undergoing phase-transitions. Among philosophers, this explanandum phenomenon is often described as the occurrence of a particular kind of multiply realized macro-behavior. In several recent publications, Robert Batterman denies that RG explanations account for this explanandum phenomenon by following (what I call) the commonality strategy, i.e. by identifying properties that microscopically very different physical systems have in common. Arguing against Batterman’s claim, I defend the view that RG explanations are in accord with the commonality strategy. 相似文献
49.
50.