首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2057篇
  免费   48篇
  2105篇
  2023年   20篇
  2022年   15篇
  2021年   31篇
  2020年   52篇
  2019年   57篇
  2018年   75篇
  2017年   73篇
  2016年   71篇
  2015年   62篇
  2014年   57篇
  2013年   214篇
  2012年   81篇
  2011年   88篇
  2010年   48篇
  2009年   57篇
  2008年   73篇
  2007年   72篇
  2006年   73篇
  2005年   67篇
  2004年   50篇
  2003年   50篇
  2002年   52篇
  2001年   37篇
  2000年   32篇
  1999年   36篇
  1998年   28篇
  1997年   14篇
  1996年   23篇
  1995年   20篇
  1994年   20篇
  1993年   14篇
  1992年   16篇
  1991年   17篇
  1989年   19篇
  1988年   22篇
  1987年   17篇
  1986年   15篇
  1985年   22篇
  1984年   23篇
  1982年   22篇
  1981年   17篇
  1980年   16篇
  1979年   19篇
  1977年   19篇
  1976年   15篇
  1974年   18篇
  1973年   21篇
  1972年   13篇
  1971年   14篇
  1970年   13篇
排序方式: 共有2105条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
941.
Prototypes (social images) have been shown to influence behaviour, which is likely to depend on the type of image. Prototype evaluation is based on (un)desirable characteristics related to that image. By an elicitation procedure we examined which adjectives are attributed to specific drinker prototypes. In total 149 young Dutch adults (18–25 years of age) provided adjectives for five drinker prototypes: abstainer, moderate drinker, heavy drinker, tipsy, and drunk person. Twenty‐three unique adjectives were found. Multilevel latent class cluster analysis revealed six adjective clusters, each with unique and minor overlapping adjectives: ‘negative, excessive drinker,’ ‘moderate, responsible drinker,’ ‘funny tipsy drinker,’ ‘determined abstainer cluster,’ ‘uncontrolled excessive drinker,’ and ‘elated tipsy cluster.’ In addition, four respondent classes were identified. Respondent classes showed differences in their focus on specific adjective clusters. Classes could be labelled ‘focus‐on‐control class,’ ‘focus‐on‐hedonism class,’ ‘contrasting‐extremes‐prototypes class,’ and ‘focus‐on‐elation class.’ Respondent classes differed in gender, educational level and drinking behaviour. The results underscore the importance to differentiate between various prototypes and in prototype adjectives among young adults: subgroup differences in prototype salience and relevance are possibly due to differences in adjective labelling. The results provide insights into explaining differences in drinking behaviour and could potentially be used to target and tailor interventions aimed at lowering alcohol consumption among young adults via prototype alteration.  相似文献   
942.
Can temporary mood influence people's communication strategies? According to Grice's cooperative principle, conversational utterances should ideally conform to the maxims of quantity, relevance, quality, and manner. Three experiments predicted and found that participants in a negative mood complied significantly better with Grice's maxims than did participants in a positive mood when using natural language to describe a previously observed social event. Experiments 2 and 3 further confirmed that mood influenced communication strategies, and not merely the encoding (Exp. 2) and retrieval (Exp. 3) of the relevant information. These findings are consistent with affect–cognition theories predicting that positive affect promotes a more internally focused and assimilative thinking and communication style, and negative mood promotes more externally focused and accommodative thinking, resulting in the closer observance of communication norms. The relevance of these findings for recent affect/cognition theories is considered, and the practical implications of the results for everyday conversational strategies are discussed. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
943.
    
James R. Lewis 《Religion》2013,43(1):166-169
  相似文献   
944.
    
Group dynamics are the elements that make group psychotherapy unique. When applied to the inpatient group, they illuminate various aspects of milieu treatment. Because of this linkage, the therapy group can be used to educate trainees about (1) systems dynamics on the psychiatric unit and their administrative implications, (2) the vicissitudes of the patient's treatment alliance with the hospital staff, and (3) the psychodynamics of severe psychopathology. In the group, individual defensive operations stand out with special clarity. Their expression is fueled by the group's dynamics. Here, internal object relations are reenacted and the defense of projective identification becomes vivid. The focus of this paper is not on the teaching of group psychotherapy itself, but rather on the use of this modality for the general training of future professionals.  相似文献   
945.
    
We present a mixed-effects location scale model (MELSM) for examining the daily dynamics of affect in dyads. The MELSM includes person and time-varying variables to predict the location, or individual means, and the scale, or within-person variances. It also incorporates a submodel to account for between-person variances. The dyadic specification can accommodate individual and partner effects in both the location and the scale components, and allows random effects for all location and scale parameters. All covariances among the random effects, within and across the location and the scale are also estimated. These covariances offer new insights into the interplay of individual mean structures, intra-individual variability, and the influence of partner effects on such factors. To illustrate the model, we use data from 274 couples who provided daily ratings on their positive and negative emotions toward their relationship – up to 90 consecutive days. The model is fit using Hamiltonian Monte Carlo methods, and includes subsets of predictors in order to demonstrate the flexibility of this approach. We conclude with a discussion on the usefulness and the limitations of the MELSM for dyadic research.  相似文献   
946.
947.
    
Alex King 《Ratio》2014,27(3):316-327
It is commonly assumed that ‘ought’ implies ‘can’, that is, that if we ought to do something, then it must be the case that we can do it. It is a frequent quip about this thesis that any account must specify three things: what is meant by the ‘ought’, what is meant by the ‘implies’, and what is meant by the ‘can’. 1 Something is missed, though, when we state the thesis in its shortened, three‐word form. We overlook what it means to do something. It is, I think, not mere coincidence that nobody has discussed this issue: It is very difficult to specify what it means to do something in the relevant sense. This paper is devoted to fleshing out one way of doing something that is a problem for the thesis.  相似文献   
948.
949.
    
This qualitative study explored the recovery experience of six women who have survived intimate partner violence. Interviews were used as an instrument to elicit the individual stories of each of these women. Qualitative analysis revealed two types of themes: process and supports. The process themes include (a) spirituality, (b) facing death, (c) future relationships and (d) hope and encouragement. The support themes include (a) professional help (effective and ineffective) and (b) informal supports (family/friends/other survivors). Implications for helping professionals include the call for increased understanding of the recovery process and the need to abandon preset agendas when working with survivors.  相似文献   
950.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号