首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   856篇
  免费   19篇
  875篇
  2024年   8篇
  2023年   14篇
  2022年   10篇
  2021年   21篇
  2020年   30篇
  2019年   37篇
  2018年   38篇
  2017年   45篇
  2016年   44篇
  2015年   38篇
  2014年   35篇
  2013年   95篇
  2012年   48篇
  2011年   42篇
  2010年   24篇
  2009年   29篇
  2008年   25篇
  2007年   23篇
  2006年   33篇
  2005年   24篇
  2004年   23篇
  2003年   18篇
  2002年   25篇
  2001年   11篇
  2000年   9篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   6篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   5篇
  1995年   3篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   6篇
  1989年   6篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   6篇
  1984年   7篇
  1983年   5篇
  1982年   7篇
  1981年   6篇
  1980年   5篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   5篇
  1976年   2篇
  1974年   5篇
  1973年   2篇
  1972年   2篇
  1971年   2篇
排序方式: 共有875条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
791.
The 'content view', in slogan form, is 'Perceptual experiences have representational content'. I explain why the content view should be reformulated to remove any reference to 'experiences'. I then argue, against Bill Brewer, Charles Travis and others, that the content view is true. One corollary of the discussion is that the content of perception is relatively thin (confined, in the visual case, to roughly the output of 'mid-level' vision). Finally, I argue (briefly) that the opponents of the content view are partially vindicated, because perceptual error is due to false belief.  相似文献   
792.
793.
Rajczi  Alex 《Philosophical Studies》2002,108(3):309-326
I argue that any theory of moral obligation must be able toexplain two things: why we cannot be thrust into a moraldilemma through no fault of our own, and why we can get intoa moral dilemma through our own negligence. The most intuitivetheory of moral obligation cannot do so. However, I offer atheory of moral obligation that satisfies both of these criteria,one that is founded on the principle that if you are required todo something, then you would be blameworthy for failing to do it.I conclude by relating these results to the current literatureon moral dilemmas.  相似文献   
794.
The aim of the present study was to explore whether the domains and facets of the five-factor model of personality predicted motivational states for avoidance and revenge following a transgression at a second temporal point distant from the original transgression. A sample of 438 university students, who reported experiencing a serious transgression against them, completed measures of avoidance and revenge motivations around the transgression and five-factor personality domains and facets at time 1, and measures of avoidance and revenge motivations two and a half years later. The findings suggest that neuroticism, and specifically anger hostility, predicts revenge and avoidance motivations two and a half years later. Findings are discussed within McCullough’s three systems of interpersonal forgiveness.  相似文献   
795.
The field of personality theory and assessment is characterized by a long-standing divide between proponents of self-report and indirect approaches to assessment. This article reviews the increasing convergence between the two sides, using recent writings on interpersonal dependency as an example domain. The assumptions of the self-report approach to personality assessment are contrasted with those of the indirect, and research using both types of assessments is reviewed. Also considered is the importance of recognizing dissociations between a particular individual's scores on self-report and indirect measures of a given component of self-concept, and the possible implications of such dissociations. It is argued that the convergence between two traditionally disparate approaches is likely to develop, in light of hypotheses, data and conclusions that bear notable similarity.  相似文献   
796.
Among cancers, colorectal (CRC) is the third most incident and the second most lethal. Although screening for the disease has been shown to be effective in reducing morbidity and mortality, screening rates remain low. Risk of disease has been shown to increase screening uptake, but different types of risk may influence intent to screen, screening in a timely manner, or participating in screening at all. A cross-sectional design was used to select a diverse sample of CRC asymptomatic patients 50 or more years of age (N=104) visiting one of three Midwestern medical clinics. Results showed a positive relationship between receipt of CRC screening and planning to screen for CRC in the future. Objective risk factors (personal/family history and having a primary care physician) were associated with CRC screening uptake and screening within the time intervals recommended by professional screening guidelines, but subjective risk did not obtain significance for screening participation. Both objective (primary care physician) and subjective risk (long-term comparative risk, knowledge) were associated with future plans to screen. Findings suggest that CRC screening behaviors may be differentially influenced by type of risk.  相似文献   
797.
798.
799.
800.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号