首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   207411篇
  免费   8867篇
  国内免费   157篇
  216435篇
  2020年   2826篇
  2019年   3497篇
  2018年   4073篇
  2017年   4377篇
  2016年   4970篇
  2015年   3970篇
  2014年   4836篇
  2013年   23571篇
  2012年   5855篇
  2011年   5335篇
  2010年   4478篇
  2009年   5061篇
  2008年   5270篇
  2007年   4868篇
  2006年   5058篇
  2005年   4870篇
  2004年   4444篇
  2003年   4000篇
  2002年   3797篇
  2001年   5016篇
  2000年   4755篇
  1999年   4169篇
  1998年   2976篇
  1997年   2776篇
  1996年   2699篇
  1995年   2526篇
  1994年   2485篇
  1993年   2433篇
  1992年   3457篇
  1991年   3262篇
  1990年   3147篇
  1989年   2993篇
  1988年   2947篇
  1987年   2847篇
  1986年   2865篇
  1985年   3144篇
  1984年   2994篇
  1983年   2699篇
  1982年   2511篇
  1981年   2466篇
  1980年   2319篇
  1979年   2803篇
  1978年   2459篇
  1977年   2375篇
  1976年   2232篇
  1975年   2481篇
  1974年   2668篇
  1973年   2584篇
  1972年   2150篇
  1971年   2021篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 6 毫秒
61.
62.
Using a Piagétan perspective, this study investigated the ways in which elementary school children perceive changes in the size of a televised image (in this case, a candy bar). The findings suggest that younger children perceive changes in image size from a medium shot to a close-up as changes in the object itself. Children's responses to changes in the televised image parallel their responses to traditional conservation tasks, but conservation of televised images occurs at a later age. In addition, children appear to use different cognitive skills to interpret how a zoom or a cut transforms the size of a televised image. When a zoom is used, children more readily perceive the object as “growing larger.”  相似文献   
63.
64.
This study examined the fairness reactions to 10 personnel selection methods in a sample of Italian students. University students (N=137) were asked to rate the favorability of these selection procedures and then to evaluate them on eight procedural justice dimensions. Work‐sample tests were the most favorably rated of the selection methods, followed by résumés, written ability tests, interviews and personal references. Graphology was perceived negatively. Opportunity to perform and the perceived face validity of selection procedures were the strongest procedural justice dimensions for predicting the process favorability ratings. The results of this study are compared with those from similar studies conducted in other countries. Similar results have been found in the various countries where such research has been conducted to date.  相似文献   
65.
66.
67.
68.
69.
Cognitive neuropsychology provides a theoretical framework and methods that can be of value in the study of developmental disorders, but the "dissociation" logic at the centre of this approach is not well suited to the developmental context. This is illustrated with examples from specific language impairment. Within the developing language system there is ample evidence for interaction between levels of representation, with modularity emerging in the course of development. This means that one typically is seeking to explain a complex pattern of associated impairments, rather than highly selective deficits. For instance, a selective impairment in auditory processing can have repercussions through the language system and may lead to distinctive syntactic deficits that are seen in written as well as spoken language. Changes in the nature of representations and in the relationships between components of a developing system mean that cross-sectional data at a single point in development may be misleading indicators of the primary deficit. Furthermore, traditional cognitive neuropsychology places a disproportionate emphasis on representational (competence) deficits, with processing (performance) deficits being relatively neglected. Methods for distinguishing these two kinds of impairment are discussed, as well as other approaches for elucidating the underlying nature of developmental disorders.  相似文献   
70.
We investigated eye‐movements during preschool children's pictorial recall of seen objects. Thirteen 3‐ to 4‐year‐old children completed a perceptual encoding and a pictorial recall task. First, they were exposed to 16 pictorial objects, which were positioned in one of four distinct areas on the computer screen. Subsequently, they had to recall these pictorial objects from memory in order to respond to specific questions about visual details. We found that children spent more time fixating the areas in which the pictorial objects were previously displayed. We conclude that as early as age 3–4 years old, children show specific eye‐movements when they recall pictorial contents of previously seen objects.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号