首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   64311篇
  免费   2404篇
  国内免费   29篇
  66744篇
  2019年   749篇
  2018年   1063篇
  2017年   1070篇
  2016年   1118篇
  2015年   799篇
  2014年   961篇
  2013年   5654篇
  2012年   1678篇
  2011年   1791篇
  2010年   1176篇
  2009年   1148篇
  2008年   1683篇
  2007年   1538篇
  2006年   1449篇
  2005年   1212篇
  2004年   1131篇
  2003年   1229篇
  2002年   1120篇
  2001年   2136篇
  2000年   2091篇
  1999年   1557篇
  1998年   713篇
  1996年   723篇
  1995年   700篇
  1994年   707篇
  1993年   660篇
  1992年   1448篇
  1991年   1357篇
  1990年   1337篇
  1989年   1206篇
  1988年   1171篇
  1987年   1171篇
  1986年   1141篇
  1985年   1204篇
  1984年   1007篇
  1983年   821篇
  1982年   659篇
  1981年   670篇
  1979年   962篇
  1978年   737篇
  1975年   891篇
  1974年   882篇
  1973年   1006篇
  1972年   871篇
  1971年   769篇
  1970年   705篇
  1969年   693篇
  1968年   861篇
  1967年   745篇
  1966年   783篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 6 毫秒
31.
Since 1970 about 80% of the research on oral communication apprehension has used one of two versions of McCroskey's Personal Report of Communication Apprehension. The original 20-item PRCA was introduced for the purpose of measuring apprehension beyond the traditional public speaking context. Although the PRCA-20 is reliable, some serious criticism of it has centered on its ability to tap interpersonal behavior outcomes. In an attempt to bolster its interpersonal applicability, five “interpersonally-oriented” items were added to form the PRCA-25. The studies reported herein demonstrate that both versions fail to possess sufficient reliable variance to be applicable to contexts other than public settings. In addition, it was found that the interpersonal items are a separate component of the PRCA, and as such, do not justify being treated equal in terms of additivity and, therefore, reduce the construct integrity of the PRCA.  相似文献   
32.
33.
In two experiments, hungry rats were given instrumental lever-press training for an appetitive reinforcer and, in addition, were exposed to another type of food which was not contingent on lever pressing. In the first experiment, exposure to each type of food was on separate days, whereas in the second experiment rats were exposed to each type of food in strict alternation within each session. Subsequently, a food aversion was conditioned to the reinforcer for the experimental group and to the non-contingent food for the control group. In both experiments, animals with an aversion to the reinforcer responded less in an extinction test than animals with an aversion to the non-contingent food. Subsequent reacquisition tests confirmed that the aversion to the non-contingent food in the control group was of comparable strength with that to the reinforcer in the experimental group. The results were discussed in terms of whether the reinforcer is encoded in the associative structure set up by exposure to an instrumental contingency.  相似文献   
34.
Although perceived health risk plays a prominent role in theories of health behavior, its empirical role in risk taking is less clear. In Study 1 (N = 129), 2 measures of drivers' risk-taking behavior were found to be unrelated to self-estimates of accident concern but to be related to self-ratings of driving skill and the perceived thrill of driving. In Study 2 (N = 405), out of a wide range of potential influences, accident concern had the weakest relationship with risk taking. The authors concluded that although health risk is a key feature in many theories of health behavior and a central focus for researchers and policy makers, it may not be such a prominent factor for those actually taking the risk.  相似文献   
35.
36.
37.
38.
39.
40.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号