全文获取类型
收费全文 | 476篇 |
免费 | 23篇 |
专业分类
499篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 7篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 20篇 |
2017年 | 12篇 |
2016年 | 14篇 |
2015年 | 6篇 |
2014年 | 17篇 |
2013年 | 67篇 |
2012年 | 18篇 |
2011年 | 13篇 |
2010年 | 13篇 |
2009年 | 5篇 |
2008年 | 15篇 |
2007年 | 14篇 |
2006年 | 13篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 13篇 |
2003年 | 20篇 |
2002年 | 12篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 11篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 10篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1967年 | 3篇 |
1955年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有499条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
Wilfong Kevin M. Goodie Jeffrey L. Curry Justin C. Hunter Christopher L. Kroke Phillip C. 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2022,29(2):318-331
Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings - Limited scalability combined with limited opportunities for patients to receive evidence-based interventions in traditional behavioral health... 相似文献
12.
13.
Animal Cognition - Tversky and Kahneman introduced the term framing for the finding that people give different answers to the same question depending on the way it is posed. One form of framing... 相似文献
14.
15.
Alasdair Cochrane 《Res Publica》2007,13(3):293-318
This paper examines whether non-human animals have a moral right not to be experimented upon. It adopts a Razian conception
of rights, whereby an individual possesses a right if an interest of that individual is sufficient to impose a duty on another.
To ascertain whether animals have a right not to be experimented on, three interests are examined which might found such a
right: the interest in not suffering, the interest in staying alive, and the interest in being free. It is argued that while
the first two of these interests are sufficient to ground animal rights against being killed and made to suffer by experiments,
the interest in freedom does not ground a general animal right not to be used in experimentation.
Winner of the second annual Res Publica Postgraduate Essay Prize, 2006. 相似文献
16.
Prejudice against overweight people is rife. However, there is a paucity of research on the underlying reasons for it. In two studies the relationship between body image, the tendency to make physical appearance-related comparisons (PACS), and both explicit and implicit anti-fat attitudes was examined. In Study 1 (n = 227) people with a high tendency to make physical appearance-related comparisons (high PACS scorers) reported lower self-appearance evaluation, but higher appearance orientation and explicit anti-fat attitudes. The PACS fully mediated the relationship between appearance orientation and explicit anti-fat attitudes. Study 2 (n = 134) found that the PACS also mediated the relationship between appearance orientation and implicit anti-fat attitudes. Thus, individual differences in factors such as body image and the tendency to make appearance-related comparisons, appear to play a central role in both explicit and implicit anti-fat attitudes. 相似文献
17.
Mary Hunter Jesús Rosales‐Ruiz 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》2019,111(3):449-464
During shaping, if the organism is engaged in behaviors other than the current approximation, the amount of time between reinforcers increases. In these situations, the shaper may resort to what is referred to as a “desperation‐driven click.” That is, after a period of no reinforcement, the shaper delivers one reinforcer for a nontarget approximation. Reports from professional animal trainers suggest that the animal may continue performing this new behavior, even if it is reinforced only once. This study attempted to model this phenomenon with college students. Results from the study demonstrated that a desperation‐driven click situation can be reliably produced in a controlled setting. When participants received one reinforcer for interacting with a new object following a period of no reinforcement, they interacted with the new object for a longer or equal amount of time as compared to an object that had a longer history of reinforcement. The results of this study have implications for the understanding of how reinforcement controls behavior. 相似文献
18.
Alasdair Urquhart 《Journal of Philosophical Logic》1993,22(5):449-479
Craig's interpolation theorem fails for the prepositional logicsE of entailment,R of relevant implication andT of ticket entailment, as well as in a large class of related logics. This result is proved by a geometrical construction, using the fact that a non-Arguesian projective plane cannot be imbedded in a three-dimensional projective space. The same construction shows failure of the amalgamation property in many varieties of distributive lattice-ordered monoids. 相似文献
19.
A. Hunter Threadgill 《Cognition & emotion》2019,33(2):332-345
Emotions influence cognitive processes involved in memory. While some research has suggested that cognitive scope is determined by affective valence, recent models of emotion–cognition interactions suggest that motivational intensity, rather than valence, influences these processes. The present research was designed to clarify how negative affects differing in motivational intensity impact memory for centrally or peripherally presented information. Experiments 1 & 2 found that, relative to a neutral condition, high intensity negative affect (anger) enhances memory for centrally presented information. Experiment 3 replicated this effect using another high intensity negative affect (threat). Experiment 4 extended this by finding that, relative to a neutral condition, low intensity negative affect (sadness) enhanced memory for peripherally presented information. Finally, in Experiment 5, the effects of sadness and threat on scope of memory were directly compared, finding that threat narrowed scope of memory, while sadness broadened scope of memory. Together, these results provide additional support for the motivational dimensional model of cognitive scope, in that high intensity emotions narrow cognitive scope, while low intensity emotions broaden cognitive scope. 相似文献
20.
Participants use simple contextual cues to reduce deployment of costly monitoring processes in contexts in which prospective memory (PM) targets are not expected. This study investigated whether this strategic monitoring pattern is observed in response to complex and probabilistic contextual cues. Participants performed a lexical decision task in which words or nonwords were presented in upper or lower locations on screen. The specific condition was informed that PM targets (“tor” syllable) would occur only in words in the upper location, whereas the nonspecific condition was informed that targets could occur in any location or word type. Context was blocked such that word type and location changed every 8 trials. In Experiment 1, the specific condition used the complex contextual cue to reduce monitoring in unexpected contexts relative to the nonspecific condition. This pattern largely was not evidenced when the complex contextual cue was probabilistic (Experiment 2). Experiment 3 confirmed that strategic monitoring is observed for a complex cue that is deterministic, but not one that is probabilistic. Additionally, Experiments 1 and 3 demonstrated a disadvantage associated with strategic monitoring—namely, that the specific condition was less likely to respond to a PM target in an unexpected context. Experiment 3 provided evidence that this disadvantage is attributable to impaired noticing of the target. The novel findings suggest use of a complex contextual cue per se is not a boundary condition for the strategic, context-specific allocation of monitoring processes to support prospective remembering; however, strategic monitoring is constrained by the predictive utility of the complex contextual cue. 相似文献