全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2901篇 |
免费 | 98篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
3000篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 18篇 |
2020年 | 25篇 |
2019年 | 40篇 |
2018年 | 53篇 |
2017年 | 59篇 |
2016年 | 62篇 |
2015年 | 39篇 |
2014年 | 61篇 |
2013年 | 308篇 |
2012年 | 89篇 |
2011年 | 124篇 |
2010年 | 77篇 |
2009年 | 58篇 |
2008年 | 100篇 |
2007年 | 100篇 |
2006年 | 119篇 |
2005年 | 109篇 |
2004年 | 110篇 |
2003年 | 106篇 |
2002年 | 97篇 |
2001年 | 29篇 |
2000年 | 47篇 |
1999年 | 42篇 |
1998年 | 60篇 |
1997年 | 47篇 |
1996年 | 36篇 |
1995年 | 39篇 |
1994年 | 52篇 |
1993年 | 46篇 |
1992年 | 38篇 |
1991年 | 32篇 |
1990年 | 37篇 |
1989年 | 34篇 |
1988年 | 29篇 |
1987年 | 35篇 |
1986年 | 34篇 |
1985年 | 26篇 |
1984年 | 46篇 |
1983年 | 43篇 |
1982年 | 38篇 |
1981年 | 50篇 |
1980年 | 38篇 |
1979年 | 43篇 |
1978年 | 32篇 |
1977年 | 31篇 |
1976年 | 30篇 |
1975年 | 36篇 |
1974年 | 29篇 |
1973年 | 19篇 |
1972年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有3000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
A survey investigated incidences where individuals intentionally hid objects but later forgot the item's location. Surveys from 111 younger and 99 older adults yielded the following prototypical scenario: the person hid the valuable 1 year ago to prevent theft, and waited 1 month before an initial 30-minute search, which included looking in dresser drawers plus 1.5 other locations. The person made three additional searches (total searches = 4) for the missing valuable over a 2 week period. Three-quarters of the hidden valuables were eventually found (usually by accident) about 1 month after hiding the object. About half of the time, the person had already searched the hiding area. Older adults typically hid jewellery from thieves, while younger adults hid money from friends and relatives. Older adults searched more often for longer periods of time for the hidden object, and took longer to eventually find the object, compared with younger adults. 相似文献
62.
Journal of Behavioral Education - 相似文献
63.
In previous research, replicated here, we found that some object recognition processes influence figure-ground organization. We have proposed that these object recognition processes operate on edges (or contours)detected early in visual processing, rather than on regions. Consistent with this proposal, influences from object recognition on figure-ground organization were previously observed in both pictures and stereograms depicting regions of different luminance, but not in randomdot stereograms, where edges arise late in processing (Peterson & Gibson, 1993). In the present experiments, we examined whether or not two other types of contours—outlines and subjective contours—enable object recognition influences on figure-ground organization. For both types of contours we observed a pattern of effects similar to that originally obtained with luminance edges. The results of these experiments are valuable for distinguishing between alternative views of the mechanisms mediating object recognition influences on figure-ground organization. In addition, in both Experiments 1 and 2, fixated regions were seen as figure longer than nonfixated regions, suggesting that fixation location must be included among the variables relevant to figure-ground organization. 相似文献
64.
65.
Preeti G. Suppal Jaipaul L. Roopnarine Trace Buesig Alan Bennett 《International journal of psychology》1996,31(1):29-37
Ninety-two dual-wage and 103 single-wage Indian families with a preschool-age child residing in extended and nuclear households provided assessments regarding their beliefs about the division of household chores, financial responsibilities, childcare, and filial obligations. Analysis focused on possible differences in belief structures that may be attributed to the gender of the parent, family structural living arrangement, and maternal employment outside of the home. Husbands and wives did not differ in ideological beliefs on any of the components assessed, but differences did emerge as a function of family structural arrangements and whether or not the wife worked outside of the home. Data are discussed in terms of possible changes in ideological beliefs about family functions and practices and the broader socio-cultural factors operating within Indian society. 相似文献
66.
The Trace-Deletion Hypothesis (henceforth TDH; Grodzinsky 1986, 1995) states that syntactic traces are deleted in agrammatism and that whenever a trace is deleted, a default strategy is activated. The default strategy assigns the role of Agent to the first NP. In structures where a second NP receives the Agent role syntactically, the consequence is that the agrammatic representation contains two conflicting Agents for the same action. This is the mechanism that induces guessing and the random performance on comprehension tests that has often been observed for passives and certain other structures. In this paper, we isolate the default strategy of the TDH, using a sentence-picture matching task in which one of the pictures matches the meaning arrived at by the default strategy. Our results show that an agrammatic representation does not involve double-Agents, and thus the default strategy (and therefore the TDH) is refuted. 相似文献
67.
68.
69.
70.
Alan Blum 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》1985,6(2):221-232
A perspective is developed for approaching affliction as a social fact. Disability and disease are considered as two ways in which we suffer a disjunction which arises from the need to take initiative with respect to the inexorable, whether that means the mark of disability or the unconquerability of disease. The story of affliction always raises and masks in certain respects the problem of suffering as the collective representation of our experience of subjectivity where that experience passes through the separateness of being marked to the singularity of being without hope. 相似文献