首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2601篇
  免费   78篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2680篇
  2021年   13篇
  2020年   16篇
  2019年   36篇
  2018年   42篇
  2017年   52篇
  2016年   50篇
  2015年   36篇
  2014年   51篇
  2013年   261篇
  2012年   77篇
  2011年   112篇
  2010年   69篇
  2009年   58篇
  2008年   95篇
  2007年   91篇
  2006年   106篇
  2005年   99篇
  2004年   103篇
  2003年   100篇
  2002年   94篇
  2001年   26篇
  2000年   38篇
  1999年   41篇
  1998年   60篇
  1997年   45篇
  1996年   34篇
  1995年   39篇
  1994年   42篇
  1993年   43篇
  1992年   36篇
  1991年   28篇
  1990年   33篇
  1989年   30篇
  1988年   23篇
  1987年   27篇
  1986年   34篇
  1985年   25篇
  1984年   44篇
  1983年   41篇
  1982年   39篇
  1981年   48篇
  1980年   38篇
  1979年   42篇
  1978年   31篇
  1977年   32篇
  1976年   30篇
  1975年   37篇
  1974年   27篇
  1973年   18篇
  1972年   11篇
排序方式: 共有2680条查询结果,搜索用时 7 毫秒
201.
Five experiments examine the combined effects of spatial and non-spatial cues on the latency of an orientation judgement. In Experiment 1 an interaction between position and form expectancies is observed: There is an unusual delay in response time (RT) when expectancies mismatch—that is, when an unexpected (uncued) form appears at an expected (cued) position or an expected (cued) form appears at an unexpected (uncued) position. This interaction is inconsistent with the spatial spotlight model's prediction that position and form expectancy effects will be additive. The position-form interaction is eliminated when target position discriminability is made difficult by adding a distractor item at the location not containing a target (Experiment 2) or by decreasing the validity of the positional cue (Experiment 3). Decreasing the validity of the form cue has no effect on the interaction. The mismatching expectancy interaction is not unique to combinations of position and form—it is also observed when temporal (Experiment 4) and colour (Experiment 5) expectancies are combined with form expectancies. The effects of advance knowledge of position therefore suggest no special status for location compared to other stimulus attributes. Thus, at least for combined expectancies, the notion of a uniquely spatial mental spotlight seems unjustified. It is hypothesized that expectancy interactions may reflect a hierarchical crosstalk relation between dimensions of encoding.  相似文献   
202.
203.
204.
205.
Two experiments were performed to investigate the role of syntactic and pragmatic cues on the disambiguation of noun phrases of the form VERB+ing NOUN+s, like visiting relatives, that can be interpreted as either singular or plural noun phrases. Both experiments used a self-paced reading task in which reading times were measured for two words, a verb and an adverb, immediately following the potentially ambiguous noun phrase. The interpretation of the noun phrase as singular or plural was biased by pragmatic cues in the first experiment and by syntactic cues in the second experiment. In both experiments, subjects were faster to read the adverb following the verb when the interpretation biased by the cues agreed in number with the verb that immediately followed the target noun phrase than when it did not agree with the verb. These results suggest that pragmatic cues, like syntactic cues, can be utilized rapidly in sentence processing.  相似文献   
206.
    
Alan Paskow 《Man and World》1994,27(3):309-323
  相似文献   
207.
208.
209.
210.
The type of experience involved with an object category has been regarded as one important factor in shaping of the human object recognition system. Laboratory training studies have shown that different kinds of learning experience with the same set of novel objects resulted in different perceptual and neural changes. Whether this applies to natural real-world objects remains to be seen. We compared two groups of observers who had different learning experiences with faces, using holistic processing as a dependent measure. We found that, while ordinary observers had extensive individuation experience with faces and displayed typical holistic face processing, art students who had acquired additional experience in drawing faces, and thus in attending to parts of a face, showed less holistic processing than did ordinary observers. These results converge with laboratory training studies on the role of type of experience in the development of different perceptual markers for different object categories. It is thus insufficient to categorize expertise simply in terms of object domains (e.g., expertise with faces). Instead, perceptual expertise should be classified in terms of the underlying process or task demand.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号