全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2563篇 |
免费 | 77篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
2641篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 13篇 |
2020年 | 17篇 |
2019年 | 35篇 |
2018年 | 41篇 |
2017年 | 49篇 |
2016年 | 50篇 |
2015年 | 36篇 |
2014年 | 51篇 |
2013年 | 260篇 |
2012年 | 76篇 |
2011年 | 110篇 |
2010年 | 69篇 |
2009年 | 52篇 |
2008年 | 93篇 |
2007年 | 91篇 |
2006年 | 106篇 |
2005年 | 99篇 |
2004年 | 103篇 |
2003年 | 99篇 |
2002年 | 94篇 |
2001年 | 28篇 |
2000年 | 36篇 |
1999年 | 41篇 |
1998年 | 56篇 |
1997年 | 44篇 |
1996年 | 34篇 |
1995年 | 38篇 |
1994年 | 43篇 |
1993年 | 43篇 |
1992年 | 36篇 |
1991年 | 29篇 |
1990年 | 33篇 |
1989年 | 32篇 |
1988年 | 22篇 |
1987年 | 27篇 |
1986年 | 32篇 |
1985年 | 24篇 |
1984年 | 43篇 |
1983年 | 43篇 |
1982年 | 36篇 |
1981年 | 47篇 |
1980年 | 35篇 |
1979年 | 42篇 |
1978年 | 31篇 |
1977年 | 30篇 |
1976年 | 29篇 |
1975年 | 35篇 |
1974年 | 26篇 |
1973年 | 18篇 |
1972年 | 10篇 |
排序方式: 共有2641条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
31.
Previous research has indicated that success in competitive situations is differentially perceived, considering the level of success and sex of person who succeeded. The amount of projected negative imagery of 100 college students under various degrees of a man's or woman's failure in medical school was investigated in the present study. Data indicated a pervasive tendency to downgrade unsuccessful men in relation to unsuccessful women. The possibility that subjects' responses reflect anxiety about the level of success and failure obtained, rather than anxiety about success and failure in general, is discussed.The authors thank D. F. Ricks for his helpful comments on an earlier draft of this article. Portions of this article were presented at the 6th Annual Conference for Research on Women and Education, Pacific Grove, California, December, 1980. 相似文献
32.
The influence of contextual stimuli on the conditioning and performance of responding to a discrete stimulus was examined in the US preexposure paradigm using both context shift manipulations and a measure of context conditioning. Four groups of rats received both repeated exposure to an electric shock US in one context (Context 1), and repeated nonshocked exposure to a second context (Context 2). Two additional groups of rats received exposure to these contexts, but never received shock presentations. Rats exposed to shock learned to escape from the stimuli of Context 1, but did not escape from the stimuli provided by Context 2. Rats not exposed to shock failed to escape from either context. All rats then received a single CER conditioning session in which four pairings of a 3-min noise CS and shock US were presented. Half the rats received those CS-US pairings in the excitatory Context 1, while the remaining rats received those pairings in the neutral Context 2. Finally, half the rats in each of the CER conditioning treatments received extinction test trials of the noise CS in Context 1, while the remaining rats received those test trials in Context 2. Thus, this design factorially manipulated the presence of excitatory or neutral contextual stimuli during both conditioning and testing of a discrete CS. In comparison with the two groups of rats never preexposed to shock alone, attenuation in acquisition of conditioned suppression observed during test trials occurred only when CER conditioning had been administered in the excitatory Context 1, and this effect was manifested when testing occurred in either the excitatory Context 1 or the neutral Context 2. These results support the model of R. A. Rescorla and A. R. Wagner (1972) (in A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.) Classical Conditioning II, pp. 64–99, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts) which asserts that contextual stimuli and sicrete CSs compete for limited associative strength supportable by a given US. 相似文献
33.
We examined articles with experiments published in the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis and in Behavior Analysis in Practice from 2017 through 2021 to determine how frequently procedural fidelity was assessed. When procedural fidelity was assessed, we determined how often a measure of interobserver agreement for those fidelity data was provided. We also determined how often a measure of interobserver agreement for participants' behavior was provided. Across both journals and all years, 54.7% of relevant articles provided a measure of procedural fidelity. Of them, 17.7% provided a measure of interobserver agreement for procedural fidelity. In marked contrast, 96.4% provided interobserver agreement data for participants' behavior. It is unfortunate that applied behavior analysts frequently fail to provide procedural fidelity data and, when they do, often fail to provide interobserver agreement data for the fidelity data. Reviewers for, and editors of, behavior-analytic journals are encouraged to strongly consider the relative value of procedural fidelity and agreement on procedural fidelity measures when rendering recommendations on the suitability of a given submission. 相似文献
34.
Donald J. Searls Alan L. Sternberg Lowell T. Wilson R. W. Miskimins 《Journal of Employment Counseling》1969,6(1):22-26
The experiences of the placement department of the Mental Health and Manpower Project have demonstrated that an effective team can be created to develop job opportunities for the mentally restored psychiatric patient. Composed of a vocational counselor and a placement counselor, the team is effective in securing meaningful employment and developing re-education programs in the community. While the vocational counselor provides the clinical information concerning the restored patient-client, the placement counselor serves as a more personnel oriented individual in finding the appropriate kind of employment position. The team points to the potential that the mental health center possesses in involving the general community in its programs. 相似文献
35.
Simultaneous recording from scalp and cortical electrodes in unanesthetized and unrestrained cats revealed essentially no differences between the superficial and deep recordings of the auditory evoked responses. Human recordings taken under two conditions of muscular contraction contained no major differences from 30 to 300 msec after stimulus onset. 相似文献
36.
H. Alan Robinson Ralph P. Connors G. Holly Whitacre 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1966,45(4):371-379
This annual review of job satisfaction researches is the 23rd in a series and covers reports published in 1964–65 plus earlier studies not previously dealt with in this journal. The format—findings and conclusions, topics investigated, per cent dissatisfied, current emphases, and summaries of studies—has become standard in this series of articles. 相似文献
37.
Left and right halves of a visual display were covered with inducing fields (IFs) of different lightnesses. S’s monocular gaze moved over an irreversible route from a neutral Munsell target to a CO series through either the left- or right-side IFs. For the 16 Ss there were 8 different IFs, varying from light to dark. For each of three different gray targets Munsell CO choices varied directly with the lightness of the IFs through which the gaze was routed rather than with the lightness of the total presented display. A replication with modifications is also reported. 相似文献
38.
Erratum
Overt integrity tests versus personality-based measures of delinquency: An empirical comparis 相似文献39.
40.
Researchers have increasingly advocated social and developmental psychology perspectives to studying Type A behavior to increase understanding of how it emerges and of how Type A individuals’ entry into different naturalistic social situations may exacerbate their stress reactions. As an initial step in these directions, we tested structural equation models relating components of Type A behavior in adolescents to childhood and adolescent socially problematic behaviors. Results from two high-school district cohorts (Ns = approximately 300 and 700) showed that impatience, hostility, and leadership all were related to childhood problems, but that only impatience predicted an adolescent problem (conflict with friends). 相似文献