首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   33340篇
  免费   1403篇
  国内免费   26篇
  2020年   353篇
  2019年   453篇
  2018年   638篇
  2017年   613篇
  2016年   694篇
  2015年   458篇
  2014年   574篇
  2013年   2647篇
  2012年   1055篇
  2011年   1087篇
  2010年   651篇
  2009年   699篇
  2008年   917篇
  2007年   928篇
  2006年   849篇
  2005年   745篇
  2004年   737篇
  2003年   676篇
  2002年   690篇
  2001年   1068篇
  2000年   1085篇
  1999年   807篇
  1998年   374篇
  1997年   340篇
  1995年   345篇
  1993年   330篇
  1992年   655篇
  1991年   618篇
  1990年   596篇
  1989年   568篇
  1988年   557篇
  1987年   504篇
  1986年   512篇
  1985年   552篇
  1984年   470篇
  1983年   431篇
  1982年   344篇
  1979年   510篇
  1978年   355篇
  1977年   328篇
  1976年   348篇
  1975年   397篇
  1974年   456篇
  1973年   484篇
  1972年   409篇
  1971年   348篇
  1969年   339篇
  1968年   424篇
  1967年   376篇
  1966年   363篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
881.
882.
When S looks at a visual target through prisms, adaptive shifts in reaching behavior occur even though he sees no part of his body through the prisms. These shifts are caused by a change in the judgment of the direction of gaze (oculomotor change), which in turn is caused by two secondary prismatic effects: (a) asymmetry of the visual display and (b) apparent rotation about a vertical axis of a panel or wall facing S. The “asymmetry” factor contributes 22% of the total oculomotor change, and the “rotation” effect contributes the remaining 78%. Oculomotor change is not facilitated by eye-movement activity. The adaptive oculomotor change induces a non-adaptive proprioception change about one-tenth as large as the oculomotor change. These findings are capable of accounting for the previously unexplained results reported by Wooster in 1923, and also for the current controversy about the role of reafferent stimulation in sensorymotor adaptation.  相似文献   
883.
Left and right halves of a visual display were covered with inducing fields (IFs) of different lightnesses. S’s monocular gaze moved over an irreversible route from a neutral Munsell target to a CO series through either the left- or right-side IFs. For the 16 Ss there were 8 different IFs, varying from light to dark. For each of three different gray targets Munsell CO choices varied directly with the lightness of the IFs through which the gaze was routed rather than with the lightness of the total presented display. A replication with modifications is also reported.  相似文献   
884.
Voluntary attention to one of two static objects in the peripheral field of one eye makes this object more liable to masking by a moving object in the corresponding area of the field of the other eye (Experiment 1).

Positive after images (and probably negative after images) are subject to (binocular) movement masking (Experiment 2).

Movement masking can occur in the field of either eye, but with the displays so far tried the inhibitory influence of a moving object is less in the field of the eye to which it is shown than in the field of the other eye (Experiment 3).  相似文献   
885.
When S looks at a visual target through prisms, adaptive shifts in reaching behavior occur even though he sees no part of his body through the prisms. These shifts are caused by a change in the judgment of the direction of gaze (oculomotor change), which in turn is caused by two secondary prismatic effects: (a) asymmetry of the visual display and (b) apparent rotation about a vertical axis of a panel or wall facing S. The “asymmetry” factor contributes 22% of the total oculomotor change, and the “rotation” effect contributes the remaining 78%. Oculomotor change is not facilitated by eye-movzment activity. The adaptive oculomotor change induces a non-adaptive proprioception change about one-tenth as large as the oculomotor change. These findings are capable of accounting for the previously unexplained results reported by Wooster in 1923, and also for the current controversy about the role of reafferent stimulation in sensorymotor adaptation.  相似文献   
886.
887.
888.
889.
890.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号