首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   282篇
  免费   19篇
  2022年   2篇
  2021年   2篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   6篇
  2018年   7篇
  2017年   8篇
  2016年   5篇
  2015年   11篇
  2014年   7篇
  2013年   29篇
  2012年   19篇
  2011年   20篇
  2010年   13篇
  2009年   14篇
  2008年   16篇
  2007年   11篇
  2006年   17篇
  2005年   12篇
  2004年   13篇
  2003年   11篇
  2002年   8篇
  2001年   6篇
  2000年   2篇
  1999年   4篇
  1998年   6篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   6篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   3篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   1篇
  1985年   1篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   1篇
  1981年   1篇
  1976年   5篇
排序方式: 共有301条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
191.
192.
Several two-choice reaction time experiments have compared conditions in which the two possible responses were from the same hand (same-hand pairing) or from different hands (different-hand pairing). Studies that used only the two relevant fingers on response keys reported shorter reaction times for the different-hand pairing. In other studies, two additional but irrelevant fingers were also in contact with response keys. These fingers were irrelevant in the sense that they never were required to respond. With this procedure, equivalent reaction times were found between same-hand and different-hand pairings. Reeve and Proctor (1988) recently have argued that using only two fingers results in response competition between the two fingers from the same hand, yielding shorter reaction times for the different-hand pairing condition. In contrast, when four fingers are placed on response keys, response competition should be absent for both the same-hand and the different-hand pairing conditions, resulting in equivalent reaction times. In the present work, reaction times associated with the same-hand pairing condition remained unchanged, irrespective of the number of fingers positioned on keys. In the different-hand pairing condition, reaction times were found to be longer when four fingers were used than when only two fingers rested on response keys. Thus, when four fingers are placed on keys, response competition appears to be present rather than absent. Other results showed that the response competition found in the different-hand pairing condition decreases with practice.  相似文献   
193.
Based upon the experimental literature surveyed, and given that time-keeping accuracy (i.e., constant error, absolute error, beneficial anticipation) is the appropriate performance dimension to consider, there appears to be virtually no support for the speculation that proprioception serves as an effective mediator for conscious time-keeping judgements. For the most part, individuals afforded movements during the interval to be timed did not perform significantly more precisely than did those people deprived of such time-filling movements, nor did they consistently estimate time with significantly less variability. On this basis, and because other predictions of the Input Hypothesis examined herein (left/right hand covariance, practice by movement group interaction and effect of different levels of proprioception) were not experimentally supported, the validity of the Input Hypothesis was rejected.  相似文献   
194.
This study examines changes in movement parameters and energy expense during the learning of a cyclical task performed on a “ski simulator”. Five subjects performed five training sessions (each of four 4-minute learning periods separated by a 4-minute rest) and then took a post-test one week later. The instructions were to make movements which were “as wide and frequent as possible”. The subjects were filmed on videotape and their oxygen intake was measured. Movement amplitude, movement frequency, oxygen intake, and a “movement cost” index were calculated. The results indicated a significant increase in movement amplitude and oxygen intake and a decrease in movement cost. The movement frequency of all subjects tended towards the same final value. Two simultaneous changes could occur, with large individual differences: an increase in energy expense due to the increase in the amount of work done, a decrease in energy expense due to the increase in movement economy.  相似文献   
195.
Effects of context on auditory stream segregation   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
The authors examined the effect of preceding context on auditory stream segregation. Low tones (A), high tones (B), and silences (-) were presented in an ABA- pattern. Participants indicated whether they perceived 1 or 2 streams of tones. The A tone frequency was fixed, and the B tone was the same as the A tone or had 1 of 3 higher frequencies. Perception of 2 streams in the current trial increased with greater frequency separation between the A and B tones (Delta f). Larger Delta f in previous trials modified this pattern, causing less streaming in the current trial. This occurred even when listeners were asked to bias their perception toward hearing 1 stream or 2 streams. The effect of previous Delta f was not due to response bias because simply perceiving 2 streams in the previous trial did not cause less streaming in the current trial. Finally, the effect of previous ?f was diminished, though still present, when the silent duration between trials was increased to 5.76 s. The time course of this context effect on streaming implicates the involvement of auditory sensory memory or neural adaptation.  相似文献   
196.
Building upon the self‐based model of cooperation (De Cremer & Tyler, 2005 ), the present study investigates the relationship between the five‐factor model (FFM) and cooperation. Study 1 (N = 56), an experiment conducted in the laboratory, and Study 2 (N = 116), a field study conducted in an organisational context, yielded a moderator effect between neuroticism and procedural fairness in explaining cooperation. Study 3 (N = 177) showed that this moderator effect was mediated by the self‐uncertainty and relational variables proposed by the self‐based model of cooperation. It is concluded that the FFM is useful in explaining cooperation and contributes to a better understanding of (procedural) fairness effects. Moreover, the necessity to build integrative, multi‐level models that combine core and surface aspects of personality to explain the effects of fairness on cooperation is elaborated upon. Copyright © 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
197.
Researchers studying absolute identification have long known that it takes more time to identify a stimulus in the middle of a range than one at the extremes. That is, there is an inverted-U relation between mean response time and response position. In this task, an inverted-U relation also exists between response uncertainty and response position. Similarly, an inverted-U relation between mean response time and response position has been found for psychometric measures involving questions about the self. However, psychophysicists explain these inverted-U effects differently than do self-schema researchers. We propose an integrative framework in which task constraints explain these effects. To verify the generality of these inverted-U effects, we hypothesized that they would exist in three tasks having similar constraints--in this case, tasks involving the judgment of subjective properties of faces on a Likert-type scale. Our results are consistent with this hypothesis. We discuss the relevance of the results for other applications of Likert-type scales.  相似文献   
198.
199.
Learning and, more generally, the development of cognitive processes in children has been thought to depend on the nature of the conflict induced by confrontation with others (i.e., socio-cognitive conflict). The aim of this article is to extend this notion to social-influence situations involving adolescents and young adults through the presentation of a model that explains social influence in aptitude tasks. This model takes into account the differences or similarities of the competencies of the source and target. It conceptualizes the social versus epistemic regulations of conflict in terms of an identity threat that can be induced by social comparison. To illustrate parts of the model, several experiments are briefly summarized. One demonstrates the conflict regulation dynamics involved in the similarities or differences of the sources' and targets' competencies, while the others show how social-influence dynamics are modified by the presence or absence of a threat to self-competence. This revised version was published online in August 2006 with corrections to the Cover Date.  相似文献   
200.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号