全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4126篇 |
免费 | 54篇 |
专业分类
4180篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 43篇 |
2023年 | 64篇 |
2022年 | 68篇 |
2021年 | 96篇 |
2020年 | 131篇 |
2019年 | 140篇 |
2018年 | 201篇 |
2017年 | 210篇 |
2016年 | 176篇 |
2015年 | 112篇 |
2014年 | 131篇 |
2013年 | 495篇 |
2012年 | 304篇 |
2011年 | 307篇 |
2010年 | 190篇 |
2009年 | 99篇 |
2008年 | 269篇 |
2007年 | 201篇 |
2006年 | 203篇 |
2005年 | 120篇 |
2004年 | 98篇 |
2003年 | 77篇 |
2002年 | 66篇 |
2001年 | 34篇 |
2000年 | 51篇 |
1999年 | 31篇 |
1998年 | 15篇 |
1997年 | 19篇 |
1996年 | 12篇 |
1995年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 22篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 12篇 |
1987年 | 13篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1971年 | 4篇 |
1969年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
1963年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有4180条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
291.
Work-related stress and well-being: the roles of direct action coping and palliative coping 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Fortes-Ferreira L Peiró JM González-Morales MG Martín I 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2006,47(4):293-302
The purpose of the present study is to analyze the roles of direct action coping and palliative coping in the relationship between work stressors and psychological well-being, as well as their possible interactions, in a sample of 464 bank employees. Hierarchical regression analyses showed main effects of direct action coping on well-being. Palliative coping predicts higher levels of psychological distress. Contrary to what was expected, the interactions between work stressors and direct action coping were not significant. Palliative coping interacted with work stressors when predicting psychosomatic complaints. The interaction between the two types of coping was significant on psychosomatic complaints and psychological distress, but not on job satisfaction. The paper discusses theoretical and practical implications of these results, in order to design intervention strategies to prevent and manage job stress. 相似文献
292.
Vidal-Gonzalez I Vidal-Gonzalez B Rauch SL Quirk GJ 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》2006,13(6):728-733
Recent studies using lesion, infusion, and unit-recording techniques suggest that the infralimbic (IL) subregion of medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is necessary for the inhibition of conditioned fear following extinction. Brief microstimulation of IL paired with conditioned tones, designed to mimic neuronal tone responses, reduces the expression of conditioned fear to the tone. In the present study we used microstimulation to investigate the role of additional mPFC subregions: the prelimbic (PL), dorsal anterior cingulate (ACd), and medial precentral (PrCm) cortices in the expression and extinction of conditioned fear. These are tone-responsive areas that have been implicated in both acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear. In contrast to IL, microstimulation of PL increased the expression of conditioned fear and prevented extinction. Microstimulation of ACd and PrCm had no effect. Under low-footshock conditions (to avoid ceiling levels of freezing), microstimulation of PL and IL had opposite effects, respectively increasing and decreasing freezing to the conditioned tone. We suggest that PL excites amygdala output and IL inhibits amygdala output, providing a mechanism for bidirectional modulation of fear expression. 相似文献
293.
The primary objective of this study was to translate and validate Situational Inventory of Body-Image Dysphoria-Short form (SIBID-S) in a Spanish population. The scale consists of 20 items to assess the frequency of dysphoric body-image emotions in certain situational contexts, using a five-point rating scale. The questionnaire was administered to 214 women between the ages of 14 and 29 years, from primary and secondary schools and a university. Principal components analysis indicated a one-factor structure for the entire sample and both younger (<18) and older (≥18) participants. Internal consistency was high (0.94), and the test–retest reliability over 1 month ranged between 0.89 and 0.93. The SIBID-S showed good convergent validity in relation to other pertinent measures of body image, eating pathology, and self-esteem. Moreover, the SIBID-S was differentiated low-risk, subclinical eating disturbed, and clinical eating disordered patients. Results support the potential utility of the SIBID-S in the Spanish female population, including a younger group than previously studied, as well as its usefulness in the assessment of body image among females varying in degrees of eating pathology. 相似文献
294.
295.
Daniel A. Fockenberg Sander L. Koole Gün R. Semin 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2006,42(6):799-806
It is well-established that affective stimuli can prime congruent evaluations if they precede the target within a short time interval, i.e., forward affective priming. The present research examines whether similar effects occur if affective primes succeed target presentation, i.e., backward affective priming. Experiments 1 found short-lived, yet reliable backward affective priming. Experiment 2 found parallel forward affective priming in the same paradigm. Experiment 3 found forward and backward affective priming in a within-subjects design. Comparison with neutral primes suggested that the observed effects were mainly due to interference. Backward affective priming is a robust phenomenon that may reflect a rapid and continuous evaluation of environmental stimuli. 相似文献
296.
The Self-Directed Search is a career-planning guide that works similarly to a vocational and professional interests inventory. A preliminary study allows detection of 68 ítems that need to be rectified, since they do not meet psychometric and semantic criteria. The current study demonstrates reliability and validity of the new adaptation. It is applied to a sample of 1460 subjects from different educational levels. As for reliability, the discrimination index of the items and internal consistency of the scales have improved. Additionally, procedures performed to verify validity (content, construct and concurrent) demonstrate that the instrument has acceptable validity. On balance, we can conclude that the instrument has sufficient scientific and technical credibility for application to the Spanish population. 相似文献
297.
Three experiments investigated the effects of two variables -selective attention during encoding and delay between study and test- on implicit (picture fragment completion and object naming) and explicit (free recall and recognition) memory tests. Experiments 1 and 2 consistently indicated that (a) at all delays (immediate to 1 month), picture-fragment identification threshold was lower for the attended than the unattended pictures; (b) the attended pictures were recalled and recognized better than the unattended; and (c) attention and delay interacted in both memory tests. For implicit memory, performance decreased as delay increased for both attended and unattended pictures, but priming was more pronounced and lasted longer for the attended pictures; it was still present after a 1-month delay. For explicit memory, performance decreased as delay increased for attended pictures, but for unattended pictures performance was consistent throughout delay. By using a perceptual object naming task, Experiment 3 showed reliable implicit and explicit memory for attended but not for unattended pictures. This study indicates that picture repetition priming requires attention at the time of study and that neither delay nor attention dissociate performance in explicit and implicit memory tests; both types of memory require attention, but explicit memory does so to a larger degree. 相似文献
298.
299.
300.