首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1395篇
  免费   57篇
  国内免费   1篇
  1453篇
  2023年   9篇
  2022年   15篇
  2021年   16篇
  2020年   29篇
  2019年   42篇
  2018年   48篇
  2017年   55篇
  2016年   48篇
  2015年   32篇
  2014年   42篇
  2013年   161篇
  2012年   63篇
  2011年   69篇
  2010年   46篇
  2009年   53篇
  2008年   65篇
  2007年   58篇
  2006年   40篇
  2005年   51篇
  2004年   36篇
  2003年   31篇
  2002年   45篇
  2001年   19篇
  2000年   20篇
  1999年   18篇
  1998年   17篇
  1997年   9篇
  1996年   10篇
  1995年   7篇
  1994年   18篇
  1992年   10篇
  1991年   7篇
  1990年   14篇
  1989年   16篇
  1988年   11篇
  1987年   14篇
  1986年   10篇
  1985年   24篇
  1984年   18篇
  1983年   17篇
  1982年   11篇
  1980年   7篇
  1979年   14篇
  1978年   7篇
  1977年   6篇
  1976年   6篇
  1975年   6篇
  1974年   6篇
  1973年   6篇
  1970年   7篇
排序方式: 共有1453条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
911.
Bewirken     
Ohne Zusammenfassung  相似文献   
912.
913.
Adaptation to convergence-altering prism spectacles was studied. The subjects were trained for 8 min with either convergence-increasing or convergence-decreasing prisms. Before and after training they estimated the distance to a binocular dot varying in the stimulus it provided to convergence. Both the preadaptation and postadaptation estimates showed that the reciprocal of estimated target distance is linearly related to convergence. Further there was a consistent change in the direction of adaptation expected from the preadaptation to the postadaptation test. This change was accounted for in terms of a recalibration of the effective absolute convergence level.  相似文献   
914.
Synthetic biology provides a vivid and richly entangled contemporary example of a science being made public. A science, however, can be made public in different ways. A public could validate, legitimate, de-legimate, object to, verify, confirm or dissent from science. Practically, scientists could publicise science—in the mass media—or they could make science public. The contrast between high-profile, media scientists such as J. Craig Venter, and community-based participatory mechanisms such as OpenWetWare allows us to see how these alternatives play out in practice. While it is easy to criticise and dismiss the public-relations oriented promotion of synthetic biology by figures such as Venter, how should we evaluate the open participatory mechanisms of a social media effort such as OpenWetWare? I suggest, drawing on the work of Isabelle Stengers and Michael Warner, that the case of synthetic biology is interesting because many synthetic biologists commit themselves to making it public, and making its public-ness part of how it is done. They place hope in publics to make the science viable. At the same time, however, the publics who are welcomed into OpenWetWare are largely confined to validating the coordination mechanisms on which the claim to public-ness rests. Whether publics can do more than validate synthetic biology, then, remains a question both for publics outside and inside this emerging scientific field. And whether the alternatives of validation or participation themselves adequately frame what is at stake in the emergence of fields such as synthetic biology remains debatable.  相似文献   
915.
916.
Etiopathogenetic models of somatoform disorders and philosophical concepts of free will refer to theories of affect processing: affective dysregulation can promote psychosomatic ailments as well as problems in decision making. This observation brings forth the question if freedom of will is impaired in persons with somatoform disorders. By compiling, analyzing and contextualizing relevant models of current neuroscience, psychosomatic medicine and philosophy the psychodynamic construct of alexithymia as an etiologic factor of somatoform disorders and the conceptual integration of affective factors in a model of free will are developed on the basis of a common concept of affect. The synthesis of the discussed results and considerations, the multidimensional impairment of the ability to decide freely and self-determinedly due to somatoform disorders, is critically reflected; however, a blanket assumption is not admissible.  相似文献   
917.
Abstract

Objective: Increased levels of dietary restraint are associated with deficits on many cognitive tasks. Less is known about how individual differences in restraint influences complex cognition such as reasoning which is the focus of this research.

Design: Two experimental studies are reported. In study 1, participants (n?=?158) completed a causal conditional reasoning task with statements about weight-related and general causal relationships. Study 2 replicated and extended study 1. Participants (n?=?108) completed a causal conditional reasoning task focusing on behavioural causes of weight change or general statements.

Main outcome measure: Causal conditional reasoning task performance.

Results: In study 1, levels of dietary restraint were negatively associated with reasoning abilities for weight-related statements only. Study 2 replicated the negative association between dietary restraint and reasoning finding the effect in both weight-related, and general, causal judgements.

Conclusion: The novel findings show that individual differences in dietary restraint have a wider relationship with cognition than previously demonstrated. Results tentatively support theoretical explanations of a reduction in cognitive capacity, rather than differences in belief, explaining reasoning deficits. These findings open an interesting avenue for research and might have implications for effective decision making about personal health behaviours, such as food choice.  相似文献   
918.
We investigate what processes may underlie heterogeneity in social preferences. We address this question by examining participants' decisions and associated response times across 12 mini‐ultimatum games. Using a finite mixture model and cross‐validating its classification with a response time analysis, we identified four groups of responders: one group takes little to no account of the proposed split or the foregone allocation and swiftly accepts any positive offer; two groups process primarily the objective properties of the allocations (fairness and kindness) and need more time the more properties need to be examined; and a fourth group, which takes more time than the others, appears to take into account what they would have proposed had they been put in the role of the proposer. We discuss implications of this joint decision–response time analysis. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
919.
The convergent and discriminant validity of two methods to assess a broad spectrum of aberrant personality tendencies was examined in a large sample of managers who were administered the NEO‐PI‐R (N = 11 862) and the Hogan Development Survey (N = 6774) in the context of a professional development assessment. Five‐Factor Model (FFM) aberrant compounds, defined as linear combinations of NEO‐PI‐R facets, converged for the antisocial, borderline, histrionic, avoidant and obsessive–compulsive tendencies with their respective Hogan Development Survey counterparts. Alternative linear FFM combinations did improve convergent results for the schizoid and obsessive–compulsive pattern. Risk for various aberrant tendencies was roughly equal across different employment sectors, with a higher prevalence of borderline, avoidant and dependent tendencies in the legal and more histrionic tendencies in the retail sector. Adopting FFM aberrant compound cut‐offs developed for coaching purposes to flag at risk individuals showed that 20% to 25% of all managers qualified for at least one and 10% to 15% were flagged as at risk for two or more aberrant tendencies. The theoretical implications and the repercussions of this research for the design of professional development and coaching trajectories are discussed. Copyright © 2013 European Association of Personality Psychology  相似文献   
920.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号