全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3982篇 |
免费 | 51篇 |
专业分类
4033篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 35篇 |
2023年 | 41篇 |
2022年 | 41篇 |
2021年 | 73篇 |
2020年 | 83篇 |
2019年 | 115篇 |
2018年 | 150篇 |
2017年 | 151篇 |
2016年 | 166篇 |
2015年 | 116篇 |
2014年 | 135篇 |
2013年 | 460篇 |
2012年 | 204篇 |
2011年 | 235篇 |
2010年 | 134篇 |
2009年 | 135篇 |
2008年 | 185篇 |
2007年 | 154篇 |
2006年 | 133篇 |
2005年 | 102篇 |
2004年 | 123篇 |
2003年 | 126篇 |
2002年 | 98篇 |
2001年 | 76篇 |
2000年 | 82篇 |
1999年 | 56篇 |
1998年 | 41篇 |
1997年 | 37篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 24篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 38篇 |
1991年 | 27篇 |
1990年 | 45篇 |
1989年 | 43篇 |
1988年 | 37篇 |
1987年 | 23篇 |
1986年 | 31篇 |
1985年 | 29篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 18篇 |
1982年 | 16篇 |
1981年 | 10篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 15篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1972年 | 11篇 |
1966年 | 6篇 |
1938年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有4033条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
771.
The theory has been misconstrued in four primary ways, which are often expressed as the claims of psychological reductionism, conceptual redundancy, biological reductionism, and hierarchy justification. This paper addresses these claims and suggests how social dominance theory builds on and moves beyond social identity theory and system justification theory. 相似文献
772.
This article concerns the question how innovation is achieved in open source communities. Open source communities are collections
of geographically distributed individuals, who volunteer or are paid to participate in the development of highly innovative
software programs. These communities are argued to be self-organizing systems in which high levels of variation are achieved.
Next, this article focuses on selection as part of innovation. It is shown that selection is achieved through a rather simple
principle of copying and following tags. Four tags are identified and discussed and it is demonstrated how they lead to the
selection of software among the great level of variety.
Ruben van Wendel de Joode is a Ph.D. student. He is part of Betade, which is one of the Delft interfaculty research centers;
and the Dutch Institute of Government (NIG), the research school for public administration and political science. His Ph.D.
research, scheduled to be finished mid 2004, focuses on open source. He has published numerous articles on OSS. 相似文献
773.
774.
This paper attempts to systematically characterize critical reactions in argumentative discourse, such as objections, critical
questions, rebuttals, refutations, counterarguments, and fallacy charges, in order to contribute to the dialogical approach
to argumentation. We shall make use of four parameters to characterize distinct types of critical reaction. First, a critical
reaction has a focus, for example on the standpoint, or on another part of an argument. Second, critical reactions appeal to some kind of norm, argumentative or other. Third, they each have a particular illocutionary force, which may include that of giving strategic advice to the other. Fourth, a critical reaction occurs at a particular level of dialogue (the ground level or some meta-level). The concepts here developed shall be applied to discussions of critical
reactions by Aristotle and by some contemporary authors. 相似文献
775.
Pieter T.M. Desmet David De Cremer Eric van Dijk 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》2011,114(2):75-86
Trust is vital yet vulnerable in economic exchange relations. In these relations, a widely used strategy in response to distributive harm consists of having the transgressor pay a financial compensation to the victim. This research examines whether financial compensations can increase trust towards a transgressor, and whether the size of the compensation is relevant to this process. We hypothesized and found that whether larger compensations will elicit more trust, depends on how clear the perpetrator’s intention to transgress was. Experiment 1 revealed that trust perceptions increased more by a slight overcompensation of the inflicted harm as compared to an exact or a partial compensation, but not if the transgressor’s bad intentions became clear through the use of deception in the violation. In Experiments 2 and 3, we replicated these findings and further showed that it is not the use of deception per se, but rather the attribution of bad intent that moderates the effect of compensation size. Experiment 4, using a trust game paradigm revealed that this effect not only occurs for small overcompensations, but also for larger overcompensations. 相似文献
776.
Brain imaging studies suggest that truth telling constitutes the default of the human brain and that lying involves intentional suppression of the predominant truth response. By manipulating the truth proportion in the Sheffield lie test, we investigated whether the dominance of the truth response is malleable. Results showed that frequent truth telling made lying more difficult, and that frequent lying made lying easier. These results implicate that (1) the accuracy of lie detection tests may be improved by increasing the dominance of the truth response and that (2) habitual lying makes the lie response more dominant. 相似文献
777.
Statistical properties in the visual environment can be used to improve performance on visual working memory (VWM) tasks. The current study examined the ability to incidentally learn that a change is more likely to occur to a particular feature dimension (shape, color, or location) and use this information to improve change detection performance for that dimension (the change probability effect). Participants completed a change detection task in which one change type was more probable than others. Change probability effects were found for color and shape changes, but not location changes, and intentional strategies did not improve the effect. Furthermore, the change probability effect developed and adapted to new probability information quickly. Finally, in some conditions, an improvement in change detection performance for a probable change led to an impairment in change detection for improbable changes. 相似文献
778.
Stigma is a common phenomenon worldwide and infectious diseases like HIV/AIDS and leprosy are often associated with high levels of stigma. Several studies have been conducted concerning the effects of stigma and the impact on social participation, but comparative studies are rare. The objective of this study was to identify differences and similarities between HIV/AIDS and leprosy-related stigma. From April till July 2009, 190 questionnaire-based interviews were conducted to assess the levels of internalized stigma (Internalized Stigma of Mental Illness scale), perceived stigma (Explanatory Model Interview Catalogue stigma scale) and social participation (Participation scale) in a cross-sectional sample of people affected by leprosy (PL) and people living with HIV/AIDS (PLHA). Respondents were selected from several hospitals, charity projects and during home visits in Vellore district, Tamil Nadu. Our results showed that both PLHA (n?=?95) and leprosy-affected respondents (n?=?95) faced a substantial burden of internalized and perceived stigma, with the former reporting a significantly higher level of stigma. As a result, PLHA faced more frequent and also more severe participation restrictions than PL. Especially, restrictions in work-related areas were reported by the majority of the respondents. In conclusion, PLHA faced a significantly higher level of stigma and participation restriction than PL. However, the latter also reported a substantial burden of stigma and participation restrictions. The study suggests that it may be possible to develop joint interventions based on the commonalities found. More research is needed to define these more precisely and to test the effectiveness of such joint interventions in reducing stigma and improving social participation. 相似文献
779.
We investigated whether mindfulness training (MT) influences information processing in a working memory task with complex
visual stimuli. Participants were tested before (T1) and after (T2) participation in an intensive one-month MT retreat, and
their performance was compared with that of an age- and education-matched control group. Accuracy did not differ across groups
at either time point. Response times were faster and significantly less variable in the MT versus the control group at T2.
Since these results could be due to changes in mnemonic processes, speed–accuracy trade-off, or nondecisional factors (e.g.,
motor execution), we used a mathematical modeling approach to disentangle these factors. The EZ-diffusion model (Wagenmakers,
van der Maas, & Grasman, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 14:(1), 3–22, 2007) suggested that MT leads to improved information quality and reduced response conservativeness, with no changes in nondecisional
factors. The noisy exemplar model further suggested that the increase in information quality reflected a decrease in encoding
noise and not an increase in forgetting. Thus, mathematical modeling may help clarify the mechanisms by which MT produces
salutary effects on performance. 相似文献
780.
This special issue of the journal‘Studies in Logic’ results from an international workshopon‘TheHistory of Logic in China’held in Amsterdam,The Netherlands,on November 24-26, 2010 (http://www.sciencehistory.asia/history-logic-china).This event brought together, 相似文献