首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   16951篇
  免费   795篇
  国内免费   7篇
  17753篇
  2020年   226篇
  2019年   227篇
  2018年   342篇
  2017年   299篇
  2016年   351篇
  2015年   279篇
  2014年   331篇
  2013年   1296篇
  2012年   591篇
  2011年   532篇
  2010年   380篇
  2009年   349篇
  2008年   486篇
  2007年   482篇
  2006年   408篇
  2005年   402篇
  2004年   379篇
  2003年   312篇
  2002年   339篇
  2001年   693篇
  2000年   679篇
  1999年   463篇
  1998年   179篇
  1997年   168篇
  1996年   169篇
  1992年   365篇
  1991年   344篇
  1990年   360篇
  1989年   307篇
  1988年   290篇
  1987年   283篇
  1986年   258篇
  1985年   257篇
  1984年   242篇
  1983年   205篇
  1982年   161篇
  1979年   224篇
  1978年   176篇
  1977年   151篇
  1976年   140篇
  1975年   195篇
  1974年   234篇
  1973年   232篇
  1972年   184篇
  1971年   181篇
  1970年   149篇
  1969年   177篇
  1968年   214篇
  1967年   192篇
  1966年   157篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
901.
902.
Thirty-three twelve-month-old infants were divided into three groups based on the frequency and length of naturally-occurring nontraumatic separations from mother during the first year. One group had experienced only brief separations when left with a babysitter for an evening. A second group had been left once or twice for an average of five days while parents vacationed. A third group had been left regularly and frequently during the daytime, usually because mother was working. These groups were compared on measures of reaction to separation and reunion, in the home and in the laboratory, at one year of age. No group differences were found on any of the dependent variables. It is suggested that the quality of substitute care is probably more important than separation per se in mediating the impact of employment or daycare on infant development.  相似文献   
903.
904.
A model was presented describing the reciprocal influence of disruptions in parent discipline practices on irritable exchanges between the target child and other family members. Disrupted parent discipline and irritable microsocial exchanges within the family were hypothesized to provide a basic training for aggression that generalizes to other settings such that the child is identified by peers, teachers, and parents as physically aggressive. Physical fighting was thought to lead to rejection by the normal peer group, which was hypothesized to feed back to further exacerbate fighting. Multilevel assessment including interview, questionnaires, laboratory studies, and home observations were carried out with the families of 91 preadolescent and adolescent boys. Nine indicators from the assessment battery were used to define the constructs Inept Parental Discipline, Negative Microsocial Exchanges, Physical Fighting, and Poor Peer Relations. Structural equations (LISREL VI) were used to describe the relations among the constructs. The t values for the path coefficients were significant. A chi-square analysis showed an acceptable fit between the model and the empirical findings. The findings were interpreted as being consistent with the hypothesis that under certain circumstances, family interaction may serve as basic training for aggression. In the present study, interactions with siblings in the home seemed to serve a pivotal role.  相似文献   
905.
906.
This study used the interpersonal–psychological theory of suicide to explore the relationships among DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptom clusters derived from the six-factor anhedonia model and facets of acquired capability for suicide (ACS). In a sample of 373 trauma-exposed undergraduates, most PTSD symptom clusters were negatively associated with facets of ACS in bivariate correlations, but the anhedonia cluster was positively associated with ACS in regression models. Structure coefficients and commonality analysis indicated that anhedonia served as a suppressor variable for the other symptom clusters. Our findings further elucidate the complex relationship between specific PTSD symptom clusters and ACS.  相似文献   
907.
In recent years, an impressive research program has developed around non‐analytic reductions of the normative. Nevertheless, non‐analytic naturalists face a damning dilemma: either they need to give the same reductive analysis for epistemic and practical reasons, or they can give a different analyses by treating epistemic and practical reasons as a species of the larger genus, reasonhood. Since, for example, a desire‐based account of epistemic reasons is implausible, the reductionist must opt for the latter. Yet, if the desire‐based account of practical reasons is merely a species of the larger genus, then, due to a violation of irreflexivity, the reduction fails.  相似文献   
908.
909.
910.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号