全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1610篇 |
免费 | 123篇 |
专业分类
1733篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 12篇 |
2022年 | 19篇 |
2021年 | 25篇 |
2020年 | 61篇 |
2019年 | 56篇 |
2018年 | 82篇 |
2017年 | 67篇 |
2016年 | 88篇 |
2015年 | 73篇 |
2014年 | 76篇 |
2013年 | 212篇 |
2012年 | 126篇 |
2011年 | 103篇 |
2010年 | 82篇 |
2009年 | 79篇 |
2008年 | 89篇 |
2007年 | 64篇 |
2006年 | 44篇 |
2005年 | 43篇 |
2004年 | 68篇 |
2003年 | 42篇 |
2002年 | 39篇 |
2001年 | 21篇 |
2000年 | 13篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 10篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有1733条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
Joshua D. Miller W. Keith Campbell Diana L. Young Chad E. Lakey Dennis E. Reidy Amos Zeichner Adam S. Goodie 《Journal of personality》2009,77(3):761-794
ABSTRACT A recent meta-analysis ( S. Vazire & D. C. Funder, 2006 ) suggested that narcissism and impulsivity are related and that impulsivity partially accounts for the relation between narcissism and self-defeating behaviors (SDB). This research examines these hypotheses in two studies and tests a competing hypothesis that Extraversion and Agreeableness account for this relation. In Study 1 , we examined the relations among narcissism, impulsivity, and aggression. Both narcissism and impulsivity predicted aggression, but impulsivity did not mediate the narcissism–aggression relation. In Study 2 , narcissism was related to a measure of SDB and manifested divergent relations with a range of impulsivity traits from three measures. None of the impulsivity models accounted for the narcissism–SDB relation, although there were unique mediating paths for traits related to sensation and fun seeking. The domains of Extraversion and low Agreeableness successfully mediated the entire narcissism–SDB relation. We address the discrepancy between the current and meta-analytic findings. 相似文献
92.
Brad J. Nakamura Chad Ebesutani Adam Bernstein Bruce F. Chorpita 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2009,31(3):178-189
The Child Behavior Checklist for Ages 6–18 (CBCL/6-18) possesses newly developed DSM-Oriented Scales, constructed through
expert clinical judgment to match selected categories for behavioral/emotional problems as described in the DSM-IV. The present
investigation examined the basic psychometric properties for all six DSM-Oriented Scales (i.e., Affective, Anxiety, Somatic,
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity, Oppositional, and Conduct Scales) in a large clinical sample of children and adolescents
(N = 673). Findings from the present study provide strong evidence for the reliability, as well as convergent and discriminative
validity, of these scales. It appears that the DSM-Oriented Scales may provide accurate supplementary information that may
be considered when formulating clinical diagnoses.
相似文献
Brad J. NakamuraEmail: |
93.
This compound paper presents the views of two Polish philosophers on the strong international pressures influencing the development of Polish philosophy in recent times. The first part, by Leszek Koczanowicz, treats the philosophical situation and problems of totalitarian Poland under the influence of Soviet Marxism, while the second part, by Adam Chmielewski, focuses on the main trends and difficulties of post-totalitarian Poland, dominated by Western influence. 相似文献
94.
Both humans and non-human animals have the ability to navigate and make decisions within complex environments. This ability is largely dependent upon learning and memory processes, many of which are known to depend on NMDA-sensitive receptors. When humans come to difficult decisions they often pause to deliberate over their choices. Similarly, rats pause at difficult choice points. This behavior, known as vicarious trial and error (VTE), is hippocampally dependent and entails neurophysiological representations of expectations of future outcomes in hippocampus and downstream structures. In order to determine the dependence of VTE behaviors on NMDA-sensitive receptors, we tested rats on a Multiple-T choice task with a reward-delivery reversal known to elicit VTE. Rats under the influence of NMDA-receptor antagonists (CPP) showed a significant reduction in VTE, particularly at the reward reversal, implying a role for NMDA-sensitive receptors in the generation of vicarious trial and error behaviors. 相似文献
95.
Knight JB Meeks JT Marsh RL Cook GI Brewer GA Hicks JL 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2011,37(2):298-307
In event-based prospective memory, current theories make differing predictions as to whether intention-related material can be spontaneously noticed (i.e., noticed without relying on preparatory attentional processes). In 2 experiments, participants formed an intention that was contextually associated to the final phase of the experiment, and lures that overlapped to differing degrees with the features of the intention-related cues were embedded in the initial phase. When participants were outside of the appropriate responding context (i.e., the initial phase), they exhibited slower latencies to lures that exactly matched the features of their intention compared with other types of lures and control words. In addition, on a final remember/know recognition test, participants reported having greater subjective recollection for the occurrence of the exact-match lures. These results suggest that exact-match lures were spontaneously noticed and differentially processed in the absence of any observable preparatory attentional processes. The findings have implications for the theoretical debate over whether preparatory attention must always be relied upon to notice intention-related material. 相似文献
96.
97.
John W. Philbeck Adam J. Woods Carly Kontra Petra Zdenkova 《Behavior research methods》2010,42(1):148-160
Blindwalking has become a common measure of perceived absolute distance and location, but it requires a relatively large testing
space and cannot be used with people for whom walking is difficult or impossible. In the present article, we describe an alternative
response type that is closely matched to blindwalking in several important respects but is less resource intensive. In the
blindpulling technique, participants view a target, then close their eyes and pull a length of tape or rope between the hands to indicate
the remembered target distance. As with blindwalking, this response requires integration of cyclical, bilateral limb movements
over time. Blind-pulling and blindwalking responses are tightly linked across a range of viewing conditions, and blind-pulling
is accurate when prior exposure to visually guided pulling is provided. Thus, blindpulling shows promise as a measure of perceived
distance that may be used in nonambulatory populations and when the space available for testing is limited. 相似文献
98.
Adam Kadlac 《Ethical Theory and Moral Practice》2010,13(4):421-437
This paper explores the debate between personists, who argue that the concept of a person if of central importance for moral
thought, and personists, who argue that the concept of a human being is of greater moral significance. On the one hand, it
argues that normative naturalism, the most ambitious defense of the humanist position, fails to identify moral standards with
standards of human behavior and thereby fails to undermine the moral significance of personhood. At the same time, it contends
that a more focused attention on the morally relevant features of human life may indeed play a crucial role in enhancing our
moral understanding. 相似文献
99.
Diana C. Dolan Daniel J. Taylor Adam D. Bramoweth 《Behaviour research and therapy》2010,48(4):321-327
Cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia (CBTi) has demonstrated considerable efficacy within randomized clinical trials and case-series designs. This case-series study in a community sleep medicine clinic assessed the effectiveness of an eight-session CBTi protocol chronic insomnia patients who were allowed to continue their use of hypnotics (intent-to-treat n = 48), administered by a clinical psychology doctoral student receiving training and supervision in CBTi by a behavioral sleep medicine certified clinician. Outcome measures included daily sleep diaries, self-report measures on insomnia severity, dysfunctional beliefs and attitudes about sleep, daytime sleepiness, as well as medication usage. Patients showed significant improvements in sleep onset latency, wake time after sleep onset, sleep efficiency, insomnia severity, and dysfunctional sleep beliefs from pre- to post-treatment. No changes were seen in daytime sleepiness - patients were not excessively sleepy either before or after treatment. Use of sleep medication declined significantly from 87.5% pre-treatment to 54% post-treatment, despite no active efforts to encourage patients to withdraw. Results demonstrate that a CBTi conducted in a community sleep medicine clinic with patients not required to discontinue sleep-related medications can have similar effects as therapy delivered among those not on medication. 相似文献
100.
Gillian Ku Cynthia S. Wang Adam D. Galinsky 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2010,46(5):792-798
In contrast to the view that social perception has symmetric effects on judgments and behavior, the current research explored whether perspective-taking leads stereotypes to differentially affect judgments and behavior. Across three studies, perspective-takers consistently used stereotypes more in their own behavior while simultaneously using them less in their judgments of others. After writing about an African-American, perspective-taking tendencies were positively correlated with aggressive behavior but negatively correlated with judging others as aggressive. Similarly, after writing about an elderly man, perspective-takers walked more slowly and became more conservative, but judged others as less dependent. These divergent effects of perspective-taking on judgment and behavior occurred regardless of whether perspective-taking was manipulated or measured, whether judgments were measured before or after behavior, the stereotype that was activated, and participants’ culture (American, Singaporean). These findings support theorizing that judgments and behavior can diverge when individuals’ social strategies are geared towards establishing and maintaining social bonds, as well as provide insight into how perspective-taking helps individuals manage diversity. 相似文献