全文获取类型
收费全文 | 6048篇 |
免费 | 173篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
6224篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 54篇 |
2018年 | 110篇 |
2017年 | 87篇 |
2016年 | 83篇 |
2015年 | 65篇 |
2014年 | 75篇 |
2013年 | 449篇 |
2012年 | 171篇 |
2011年 | 160篇 |
2010年 | 84篇 |
2009年 | 99篇 |
2008年 | 138篇 |
2007年 | 157篇 |
2006年 | 135篇 |
2005年 | 128篇 |
2004年 | 123篇 |
2003年 | 104篇 |
2002年 | 118篇 |
2001年 | 214篇 |
2000年 | 205篇 |
1999年 | 143篇 |
1998年 | 56篇 |
1996年 | 63篇 |
1993年 | 56篇 |
1992年 | 123篇 |
1991年 | 123篇 |
1990年 | 120篇 |
1989年 | 101篇 |
1988年 | 92篇 |
1987年 | 101篇 |
1986年 | 109篇 |
1985年 | 106篇 |
1984年 | 99篇 |
1983年 | 83篇 |
1981年 | 54篇 |
1979年 | 98篇 |
1978年 | 71篇 |
1977年 | 65篇 |
1976年 | 56篇 |
1975年 | 67篇 |
1974年 | 94篇 |
1973年 | 102篇 |
1972年 | 73篇 |
1971年 | 74篇 |
1970年 | 80篇 |
1969年 | 78篇 |
1968年 | 106篇 |
1967年 | 82篇 |
1966年 | 94篇 |
1958年 | 64篇 |
排序方式: 共有6224条查询结果,搜索用时 7 毫秒
81.
Two recent experiments (Moray, 1959; Oswald, Taylor and Treisman, 1960) have shown that in dichotic listening, and in sleep one's own name seems to be responded to selectively. We have shown for 10 subjects, each listening binaurally to the same recording of a list of their names repeated at random 10 times, and masked by noise, that the subject's own name had a significantly lower threshold than other names. The effect we found was not significantly different from the one found by Oswald et al. during sleep and by Moray for the dichotic situation.
These three experiments taken together suggest that the same pattern-analysing mechanism is involved in normal listening, dichotic listening to the rejected message, and in discrimination during sleep. 相似文献
These three experiments taken together suggest that the same pattern-analysing mechanism is involved in normal listening, dichotic listening to the rejected message, and in discrimination during sleep. 相似文献
82.
83.
84.
85.
86.
Samuel C. McLaughlin Kenneth I. Rifkin Robert G. Webster 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1966,1(5):452-458
When S looks at a visual target through prisms, adaptive shifts in reaching behavior occur even though he sees no part of his body through the prisms. These shifts are caused by a change in the judgment of the direction of gaze (oculomotor change), which in turn is caused by two secondary prismatic effects: (a) asymmetry of the visual display and (b) apparent rotation about a vertical axis of a panel or wall facing S. The “asymmetry” factor contributes 22% of the total oculomotor change, and the “rotation” effect contributes the remaining 78%. Oculomotor change is not facilitated by eye-movement activity. The adaptive oculomotor change induces a non-adaptive proprioception change about one-tenth as large as the oculomotor change. These findings are capable of accounting for the previously unexplained results reported by Wooster in 1923, and also for the current controversy about the role of reafferent stimulation in sensorymotor adaptation. 相似文献
87.
Samuel C. McLaughlin Kenneth I. Rifkin Robert G. Webster 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1966,1(12):452-458
When S looks at a visual target through prisms, adaptive shifts in reaching behavior occur even though he sees no part of his body through the prisms. These shifts are caused by a change in the judgment of the direction of gaze (oculomotor change), which in turn is caused by two secondary prismatic effects: (a) asymmetry of the visual display and (b) apparent rotation about a vertical axis of a panel or wall facing S. The “asymmetry” factor contributes 22% of the total oculomotor change, and the “rotation” effect contributes the remaining 78%. Oculomotor change is not facilitated by eye-movzment activity. The adaptive oculomotor change induces a non-adaptive proprioception change about one-tenth as large as the oculomotor change. These findings are capable of accounting for the previously unexplained results reported by Wooster in 1923, and also for the current controversy about the role of reafferent stimulation in sensorymotor adaptation. 相似文献
88.
89.
90.