首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   230篇
  免费   3篇
  2014年   2篇
  2013年   3篇
  2012年   4篇
  2011年   7篇
  2010年   8篇
  2009年   6篇
  2008年   12篇
  2006年   4篇
  2005年   2篇
  2004年   2篇
  2001年   2篇
  2000年   7篇
  1999年   3篇
  1998年   6篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   7篇
  1995年   4篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   8篇
  1991年   8篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   8篇
  1988年   7篇
  1987年   4篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   5篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   5篇
  1980年   4篇
  1979年   3篇
  1977年   4篇
  1974年   2篇
  1972年   4篇
  1969年   2篇
  1968年   2篇
  1960年   1篇
  1958年   2篇
  1957年   3篇
  1956年   4篇
  1955年   6篇
  1952年   2篇
  1951年   4篇
  1950年   3篇
  1949年   11篇
  1948年   4篇
  1946年   1篇
  1943年   1篇
排序方式: 共有233条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
21.
22.
23.
24.
Some studies have found that acknowledging one's stigmatized social identity in an evaluative context leads to more favorable evaluations, whereas others have found that stigma acknowledgment can increase negative evaluations. The present study examined one potential factor (i.e., evaluators' attitudes toward social groups to which acknowledgers belong) that may moderate the relation between stigma acknowledgment and evaluations, in the context of race and gender acknowledgment in the 2008 U.S. presidential election. Results indicated that acknowledgment of race, but not gender, led to more negative evaluations, particularly for high‐prejudiced individuals. The findings highlight the importance of examining stigma acknowledgment effects from a Person × Situation perspective. Implications for advancing understanding of acknowledgment as a useful strategy in evaluative contexts are discussed.  相似文献   
25.
26.
Böhm, B., Lundequist, A. & Smedler, A.‐C. (2010). Visual‐motor and executive functions in children born preterm: The Bender Visual Motor Gestalt Test revisited. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 51, 376–384. Visual‐motor development and executive functions were investigated with the Bender Test at age 5½ years in 175 children born preterm and 125 full‐term controls, within the longitudinal Stockholm Neonatal Project. Assessment also included WPPSI‐R and NEPSY neuropsychological battery for ages 4–7 ( Korkman, 1990 ). Bender protocols were scored according to Brannigan & Decker (2003) , Koppitz (1963) and a complementary neuropsychological scoring system (ABC), aimed at executive functions and developed for this study. Bender results by all three scoring systems were strongly related to overall cognitive level (Performance IQ), in both groups. The preterm group displayed inferior visual‐motor skills compared to controls also when controlling for IQ. The largest group differences were found on the ABC scoring, which shared unique variance with NEPSY tests of executive function. Multiple regression analyses showed that hyperactive behavior and inattention increased the risk for visual‐motor deficits in children born preterm, whereas no added risk was seen among hyperactive term children. Gender differences favoring girls were strongest within the preterm group, presumably reflecting the specific vulnerability of preterm boys. The results indicate that preterm children develop a different neurobehavioral organization from children born at term, and that the Bender test with a neuropsychological scoring is a useful tool in developmental screening around school start.  相似文献   
27.
BOOK REVIEW     
Transforming Public Policy: Dynamics of Policy Entrepreneurship and Innovation, Nancy C. Roberts and Paula J. King, ISBN 0–7879–0202–0. San Francisco: Jossey‐Bass, 1996. 280 pages, $28.95

Jungian Archetypes: Jung, Goedel, and the History of Archetypes, Robin Robertson, Nicolas‐Hays, York Beach, Maine, 1995

Sociopolitics: Political Development in Postmodern Societies, Paris Arnopoulos, Guernica Editions Inc., Toronto/New York, 1995, paper; pp. 321, index, bibliography; $20.00 Canadian, $18.00 US  相似文献   
28.
Forty young, 40 middle-aged, and 40 older adults rated their beliefs about the vocal style that they would use in addressing two older targets—one fitting a negative (Despondent) and one a positive (Golden Ager) stereotype. Participants also gave oral messages to the two targets in either a hospital or community context. Supporting a stereotype-sensitive model of the communication and aging process, participants' beliefs and messages revealed a greater tendency to use patronizing talk with the Despondent target than with the Golden Ager. In addition, the extent and type of patronizing talk (overly nurturing or directive) to the targets was affected by the context in which the target was presented and the age of the communicator. In particular, the number of directive/patronizing messages to Golden Ager targets increased significantly in the hospital context. Finally, older participants were less likely to give patronizing messages to all targets than were younger participants.  相似文献   
29.
30.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号