全文获取类型
收费全文 | 9199篇 |
免费 | 164篇 |
国内免费 | 38篇 |
专业分类
9401篇 |
出版年
2012年 | 1055篇 |
2011年 | 1204篇 |
2010年 | 241篇 |
2009年 | 118篇 |
2008年 | 912篇 |
2007年 | 863篇 |
2006年 | 799篇 |
2005年 | 655篇 |
2004年 | 602篇 |
2003年 | 545篇 |
2002年 | 489篇 |
2001年 | 298篇 |
2000年 | 374篇 |
1999年 | 143篇 |
1998年 | 30篇 |
1997年 | 26篇 |
1996年 | 27篇 |
1995年 | 13篇 |
1994年 | 24篇 |
1993年 | 25篇 |
1992年 | 30篇 |
1991年 | 31篇 |
1990年 | 26篇 |
1989年 | 27篇 |
1988年 | 33篇 |
1987年 | 25篇 |
1986年 | 25篇 |
1985年 | 28篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 24篇 |
1982年 | 19篇 |
1981年 | 21篇 |
1980年 | 15篇 |
1979年 | 17篇 |
1978年 | 22篇 |
1964年 | 12篇 |
1963年 | 17篇 |
1962年 | 12篇 |
1959年 | 16篇 |
1958年 | 60篇 |
1957年 | 58篇 |
1956年 | 64篇 |
1955年 | 49篇 |
1954年 | 38篇 |
1953年 | 33篇 |
1952年 | 29篇 |
1951年 | 35篇 |
1950年 | 23篇 |
1949年 | 33篇 |
1948年 | 20篇 |
排序方式: 共有9401条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
32.
33.
When subjects are required to produce short sequences of equally paced finger taps and to accentuate one of the taps, the interval preceding the forceful tap is shortened and the one that immediately follows the accent is lengthened. Assuming that the tapping movements are triggered by an internal clock, one explanation attributes the rnistiming of the taps to central factors: The momentary rate of the clock is accelerated or decelerated as a function of motor preparation to, respectively, increase or decrease the movement force. This hypothesis predicts that the interresponse intervals measured between either tap movement onsets or movement terminations (taps) will show the same timing pattern. A second explanation for the observed interval effects is that the tapping movements are triggered by a regular internal clock but the timing of the successive taps is altered because the forceful movement is completed in less time than the other tap movements are. This "peripheral" hypothesis predicts regular timing of movement onsets but distorted timing of movement terminations. In the present study, the trajectories of the movements performed by subjects were recorded and the interresponse intervals were measured at the beginning and the end of the tapping movements. The results of Experiment 1 showed that neither model can fully explain the interval effects: The fast forceful movements were initiated with an additional delay that took into account the small execution time of these movements. Experiment 2 reproduced this finding and showed that the timing of the onset and contact intervals did not evolve with the repetition of trial blocks. Therefore, the assumption of an internal clock that would trigger the successive movements must be rejected. The results are discussed in the framework of a modified two-stage model in which the internal clock, instead of triggering the tapping movements, provides target time points at which the movements have to produce their meaningful effects, that is, contacts with the response key. The timing distortions are likely to reflect both peripheral and central components. 相似文献
34.
Resistance To Change As A Function Of Stimulus-reinforcer And Location-reinforcer Contingencies 下载免费PDF全文
Pigeons responded on two keys in each component of a multiple concurrent schedule. In one series of conditions the distribution of reinforcers between keys within one component was varied so as to produce changes in ratios of reinforcer totals for key locations when summed across components. In a second series, reinforcer allocation between components was varied so as to produce changes in ratios of reinforcer totals for components, summed across key locations. In each condition, resistance to change was assessed by presenting response-independent reinforcers during intercomponent blackouts and (for the first series) by extinction of responding on both keys in both components. Resistance to change for response totals within a component was always greater for the component with the larger total reinforcer rate. However, resistance to change for response totals at a key location was not a positive function of total reinforcement for pecking that key; indeed, relative resistance to extinction for the two locations showed a weak negative relation to ratios of reinforcer totals for key location. These results confirm the determination of resistance to change by stimulus—reinforcer contingencies. 相似文献
35.
A theoretical analysis of the structure and dynamics of human motor functioning is presented from the perspective of a general theory of individuality. Within multifactor-systems theory, the structural units of the six systems of integrative personality (i.e., sensory, motor, cognitive, affect, style, and value) are hierarchical factor structures. Integrative functioning, both within and among systems, is described via the principles of general systems and information-processing theory. Our theoretical synthesis of the structure and dynamics of human motor functioning involves conceptualizing the hierarchy of motor factors as: (a) the basic dimensions of individual differences in the motor domain, (b) classes of decomposable motor programs that specify the spatio-temporal organization of behavior, and (c) decomposable classes of processing components entailed by motor programs. Some of the implications for understanding the role of affect in motor functioning and some research suggestions were presented. 相似文献
36.
The relative law of effect: effects of shock intensity on response strength in multiple schedules 总被引:3,自引:3,他引:0 下载免费PDF全文
Bouzas A 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1978,30(3):307-314
Key pecking of four birds was reinforced with food according to a two-component multiple variable-interval 1-minute variable-interval 4-minute schedule. In addition, key pecking was punished by a brief shock according to a variable-interval 30-second schedule during both components of the multiple schedule. The intensity of the shock was varied. For all birds, punishment had a stronger suppressive effect on the responding maintained by the leaner food schedule, and the ratio of responding during the two components of the multiple schedule became closer to the ratio of reinforcement as shock intensity was increased, as the relative law of effect predicts. At the higher shock intensity, there was some evidence that the ratio of responses overmatched the ratio of reinforcements. 相似文献
37.
鲍波尔(Koal Popper)系奥地利自然科学和社会科学哲学家。1902年出生于维也纳,在那里,他研读数学、物理和哲学。尽管他不是逻辑实证主义维也纳学派的成员,而且在许多地方同他们还有着严重的分歧,但是他的哲学志趣却和该学派的主张还是颇为投合的。他同它的一些成员也有着密切的交往,对卡尔纳普的影响尤其明显。他的第一部成名著作《研究的逻辑》出版于1935年,当时就是列为维也纳学派的科学世界观丛书而问世的。1937年,鲍波尔赴新西兰坎特堡大学执教;1945年去伦敦大学伦敦经济学院工作。自1949年他一直是该学院的逻辑学和科学方法论教授。1964年封为爵士。他 相似文献
38.
39.
40.
Violations of stochastic transitivity on concurrent chains: Implications for theories of choice 下载免费PDF全文
Houston A 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1991,55(3):323-335
The concurrent-chains procedure has been used to measure how choice depends on various aspects of reinforcement, such as its delay and its magnitude. Navarick and Fantino (1972, 1974, 1975) have found that choice in this procedure can violate the condition of stochastic transitivity that is required if a unidimensional scale for reinforcements is to be possible. It is shown in this paper that two simple unidimensional models of choice on concurrent chains can produce violations of stochastic transitivity. It is argued that such violations may result from the complex contingencies of the concurrent-chains procedure. 相似文献