首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   72228篇
  免费   2832篇
  国内免费   37篇
  2020年   745篇
  2019年   907篇
  2018年   1327篇
  2017年   1337篇
  2016年   1398篇
  2015年   958篇
  2014年   1183篇
  2013年   5980篇
  2012年   2119篇
  2011年   2297篇
  2010年   1407篇
  2009年   1397篇
  2008年   2128篇
  2007年   2060篇
  2006年   1861篇
  2005年   1656篇
  2004年   1627篇
  2003年   1518篇
  2002年   1575篇
  2001年   2228篇
  2000年   2125篇
  1999年   1656篇
  1998年   877篇
  1997年   786篇
  1996年   742篇
  1995年   733篇
  1993年   682篇
  1992年   1384篇
  1991年   1311篇
  1990年   1280篇
  1989年   1158篇
  1988年   1152篇
  1987年   1111篇
  1986年   1167篇
  1985年   1205篇
  1984年   980篇
  1983年   849篇
  1979年   987篇
  1978年   764篇
  1976年   693篇
  1975年   879篇
  1974年   988篇
  1973年   978篇
  1972年   841篇
  1971年   784篇
  1970年   725篇
  1969年   752篇
  1968年   916篇
  1967年   863篇
  1966年   820篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
881.
Forty-eight American children, 12 popular and 12 rejected children from Grades 3 and 6, were paired with same-sex friends and nonfriends on an interactive task. Children were videotaped and their interactions judged for the amount and type of attention each child in the dyad gave to each other and the conversation. Main dependent measures included mutual engagements, acknowledgement of the partner, getting the attention of the partner, attention to the conversation, and social impact of the utterances. Both rejected and popular children attended to the conversation and the partner, but rejected children appeared to overattend in several ways. The interactions of rejected children and their matches involved more mutual engagements, conversational initiators, facilitators, terminators, and nonverbal attention-getting devices. Sex and developmental effects were also found.  相似文献   
882.
883.
This study sought to identify the higher level knowledge (e.g., conservation of number) necessary for a child to understand linear measurement and to chart the growth of linear measurement in terms of the development of its components. To assess the presence of these components, a battery of 34 number, length, and distance tasks was developed and administered to 100 children between the ages of 63 and 78 months from kindergarten and Grade 1. Results indicate that there is a substantial delay between acquisition of the necessary components and emergence of a mature grasp of linear measurement. The collection of components for the number and length domains form scaled sets; within each domain, however, the pattern of development is marked by discontinuities. These discontinuities are interpreted as being associated with the need to reorganize number and length concepts. Most elements of the observed sequences of development are predicted by Piagetian theory, although others, such as asynchronies between conservation and transitive inferences of nonequivalence, are more consistent with Gagne's (1968) model of development.  相似文献   
884.
885.
886.
An assessment of the Hasher and Zacks (1979) conceptualization of the automatic nature of the acquisition of spatial location information was made in the context of manipulations of subjects' intention to learn and mood state. The Velten (1968) mood induction procedure was used to establish depressed and nondepressed subjects. Half of each group was then presented with a matrix of geometric forms under instructions to learn the location of the forms (intentional) or to evaluate the pleasantness of the array (incidental). Subsequent tests of spatial location performance found no differences as a function of subjects' mood state, but did find a strong effect for instructional condition. The superior performance of the intentional subjects raises questions about the automatic nature of the encoding of spatial location.  相似文献   
887.
888.
889.
890.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号