首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   96883篇
  免费   3988篇
  国内免费   46篇
  2020年   1122篇
  2019年   1376篇
  2018年   1953篇
  2017年   1975篇
  2016年   2061篇
  2015年   1436篇
  2014年   1746篇
  2013年   8356篇
  2012年   3187篇
  2011年   3302篇
  2010年   2000篇
  2009年   2040篇
  2008年   2959篇
  2007年   2932篇
  2006年   2605篇
  2005年   2287篇
  2004年   2318篇
  2003年   2138篇
  2002年   2121篇
  2001年   3110篇
  2000年   2966篇
  1999年   2278篇
  1998年   1165篇
  1997年   1000篇
  1996年   1010篇
  1995年   964篇
  1994年   929篇
  1993年   953篇
  1992年   1830篇
  1991年   1715篇
  1990年   1705篇
  1989年   1573篇
  1988年   1543篇
  1987年   1451篇
  1986年   1470篇
  1985年   1571篇
  1984年   1271篇
  1983年   1095篇
  1982年   868篇
  1979年   1299篇
  1978年   968篇
  1975年   1094篇
  1974年   1215篇
  1973年   1211篇
  1972年   1058篇
  1971年   949篇
  1970年   849篇
  1968年   1056篇
  1967年   993篇
  1966年   905篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Do the conditions under which promises are made determine whether they ought to be kept? Philosophers have placed a number of conditions on promising which, they hold, must be met in order to make promise-keeping obligatory. In so doing, they have distinguished valid promises from invalid promises and justified promises from promises that are not justified. Considering such conditions, one by one, we argue that they are mistaken. In the first place, the conditions they lay down are not necessary for either valid or justified promise-making. In the second place, promises need not meet such conditions in order to create moral obligations. In general, such analyses of promising fail because they suffer from a confusion between promise-making and promise-keeping. Philosophers have wrongly supposed that obligations to keep promises are dependent upon, or derivable from, the quality of the promises themselves, at the time they are made, instead of focusing on conditions that must be satisfied at the time when promises are supposed to be kept. It is not the quality of a promise that determines an obligation to keep it but the rightness or wrongness of performing the promised act.  相似文献   
72.
73.
74.
75.
76.
Several studies have illuminated how processing manual action verbs (MaVs) affects the programming or execution of concurrent hand movements. Here, to circumvent key confounds in extant designs, we conducted the first assessment of motor–language integration during handwriting—a task in which linguistic and motoric processes are co‐substantiated. Participants copied MaVs, non‐manual action verbs, and non‐action verbs as we collected measures of motor programming and motor execution. Programming latencies were similar across conditions, but execution was faster for MaVs than for the other categories, regardless of whether word meanings were accessed implicitly or explicitly. In line with the Hand‐Action‐Network Dynamic Language Embodiment (HANDLE) model, such findings suggest that effector‐congruent verbs can prime manual movements even during highly automatized tasks in which motoric and verbal processes are naturally intertwined. Our paradigm opens new avenues for fine‐grained explorations of embodied language processes.  相似文献   
77.
By roughly 6 years of age, children acquire the stereotype that men are more competent than women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM), potentially leading to greater trust in scientific information provided by men. This study tested whether 3- to 8-year-old children differentially endorsed conflicting information about science and toys presented by male and female informants depicted as a ‘man’ and ‘woman’ (Exp1) or ‘scientists’ (Exp2). Children were expected to endorse toy testimony from gender-matched informants; thus, the key question concerned endorsement of science testimony. In Exp1 (N = 149), boys and girls showed a same-gender informant preference for toy testimony; however, girls endorsed the male informant's testimony more for science than for toys – but only when tested by a male experimenter. In Exp2 (N = 264), boys and girls showed a same-gender preference, irrespective of content. Findings suggest that STEM-related gender stereotypes might lead girls to trust scientific information presented by men over women in certain contexts.  相似文献   
78.
79.
80.
陈莲笙道长生于1917年10月25日,上海市人.历任中国道教协会副会长,中国道教协会顾问,上海市道教协会会长.上海市道教协会名誉会长,上海城隍庙住持,上海道学院院长,<上海道教>杂志主编,上海市宗教学会理事,上海市政协第七届委员,上海市政协第八、九届常委等职.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号