首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   21242篇
  免费   479篇
  国内免费   219篇
  2012年   2069篇
  2011年   2296篇
  2010年   532篇
  2009年   258篇
  2008年   1822篇
  2007年   1792篇
  2006年   1663篇
  2005年   1394篇
  2004年   1190篇
  2003年   1147篇
  2002年   1024篇
  2001年   731篇
  2000年   846篇
  1999年   377篇
  1998年   119篇
  1997年   106篇
  1996年   307篇
  1995年   67篇
  1994年   128篇
  1993年   126篇
  1992年   126篇
  1991年   137篇
  1990年   141篇
  1989年   136篇
  1988年   162篇
  1987年   163篇
  1986年   133篇
  1985年   156篇
  1984年   132篇
  1983年   164篇
  1982年   114篇
  1981年   119篇
  1980年   90篇
  1979年   99篇
  1978年   95篇
  1966年   51篇
  1965年   144篇
  1964年   112篇
  1963年   134篇
  1962年   58篇
  1959年   47篇
  1958年   186篇
  1957年   156篇
  1956年   141篇
  1955年   94篇
  1954年   84篇
  1953年   81篇
  1952年   67篇
  1951年   58篇
  1949年   51篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
151.
The matching law applies to wagtails' foraging in the wild   总被引:2,自引:2,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
Field data concerning the time budgets and foraging success of pied wagtails (Motacilla alba yarrelli, Gould) are reanalyzed. It is found that the data are well described by the generalized matching law, with a marked bias towards spending time on the territory. In this case matching is not the result of maximizing reward rate, but it remains possible that it results from an allocation of time that maximizes survival.  相似文献   
152.
Three experiments describe the effects of manipulating the frequency of response-reinforcer contiguity in a recycling conjunctive schedule. The schedule arranged that a reinforcer was delivered after 30 s provided at least one response had occurred; otherwise the next cycle started immediately. In Experiment 1, this schedule produced the typical pause–respond–pause pattern, with most responses at mid-interval; and, when a limited number of contiguities between responses and food delivery were added, the pattern became more like the monotonic scallop seen on fixed-interval schedules. In Experiment 2, the schedule was initially presented with an additional contingency that allowed contiguity on every trial. Fixed-interval-like behavior occurred and tended to persist as contiguities were gradually eliminated. In Experiment 3, the recycling conjunctive schedule alternated with a condition in which a large number of contiguities occurred. The pause–respond–pause pattern and fixed-interval-like performance occurred with few or many contiguities, respectively. The results of all three experiments illustrate how contiguity interacts with a small number of other variables to determine performance on interval schedules and illuminate previous findings with fixed-interval and fixed-time schedules.  相似文献   
153.
The aim of this study was to investigate the modulations of locomotion induced by a rhythmic cognitive task (counting one's steps). Subjects (6- and 8-year-olds and adults) were requested to walk freely, and then to walk while counting their steps. Here a decrease in cadence values was observed in children only, with quasi-total repercussions on velocity at the age of 6 only. The spatiotemporal structuring of locomotion described here is already present at 6 years of age and is not altered in the step-counting situation: strong links were observed between cadence and velocity, and between stride length and velocity, and weak links between cadence and stride length.  相似文献   
154.
155.
ABSTRACT 'Ulysses contracts' are an instrument through which a psychiatric patient may prearrange involuntary commitments to be put into effect if the patient satisfies certain diagnostic criteria in the future. Proposals for Ulysses contracts typically impose numerous safeguards. This paper argues against the intuitively plausible safeguard which permits only presently remitted patients to contract. Instead of requiring a patient's remission, it is argued that the appropriate safeguard is the patient's ability, whether remitted or not, to offer good reasons for wishing to contract. In short, what matters is not an executive's character, but an executive's reasons, and a bad executive may have good reasons. Attempts to deny the accessibility of good reasons in unremitted patients are rejected on the ground that psychiatric diagnosis requires psychiatrists to be able to distinguish between good and bad reasons in both remitted and unremitted patients. If psychiatrists cannot do that, psychiatric diagnosis is impossible.  相似文献   
156.
An exploration of (I) alternative perspectives toward recent innovations in reproductive technology: support for new techniques for the sake of the kind of feminist future they facilitate; unqualified opposition despite therapeutic benefit to individual women; or qualified opposition depending upon specific threats to women's interests and (II) relationships between these positions and values bound up with mothering practices.  相似文献   
157.
社会主义社会对劳动保护一向给予应有的重视,从而能够有效地防止生产创伤的发生。为进一步减少生产创伤而挖掘潜力的一个方面,就是一定要研究在众多不幸事件的成因中占有相当比重的生产创伤的心理原因,这就需要把劳动安全心理学问题作为一个科学分支来进行研究,而建立这样一个科学分支本身就提出了很多问题。我们对于劳动安全心理学的理解最概括地说就是:“劳动安全心理是研究致创(创伤成  相似文献   
158.
世界共产主义运动发展中和现实社会主义形成中的每一个新阶段,都伴随着马克思列宁主义理论的进一步丰富,其中也包括哲学的进一步丰富,哲学是科学世界观的核心和科学地研究自然、社会和思维的方法论根据。战后数十年的显著特点是马克思主义的研究明显地趋于活跃,尤其在社会主义国家里是如此。在这种条件下,广泛研究国外马克思主义哲学家近些年所积累的理论经验,深入系统地考察他们在继续发展马克思列宁主义哲学、阐释和宣传其基本原理和原则方面的活动,就显得极为必要了。然而,在  相似文献   
159.
C.G.普拉多的《罗蒂的实用主义》一文(载《对话》杂志1983年22卷3期,本文引文均来自该文)是许多十分准确评价罗蒂著作意向的文章的一篇。他恰当地指出:罗蒂的目的在于取消哲学的传统和它所关心的问題。但是,如果普拉多以为罗蒂在这方面获得了成功,那他就错了。普拉多承认:取消  相似文献   
160.
自从1953年以来,美国心理学会(APA)就已注意到对人类被试运用解释程序的必要性。前不久,《心理学工作者道德准则》(APA,1979)正式确定了解释程序的重要性。该准则的原则9规定,“当一项研究必须采用隐瞒或说谎的方法时,研究者必须尽可能使参与者确实理解这一做法的原因和采用这种程序的正当理由”。原则9还规定:“资料搜集完毕之后,调查者要使参与者了解该研究的本质,并消除他们已产生的  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号