首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   93034篇
  免费   4888篇
  国内免费   9157篇
  2023年   959篇
  2022年   1082篇
  2021年   787篇
  2020年   911篇
  2019年   1344篇
  2018年   1666篇
  2017年   859篇
  2016年   952篇
  2015年   793篇
  2014年   1917篇
  2013年   1571篇
  2012年   7853篇
  2011年   9530篇
  2010年   3609篇
  2009年   3045篇
  2008年   8421篇
  2007年   8047篇
  2006年   7383篇
  2005年   6200篇
  2004年   5508篇
  2003年   5095篇
  2002年   4518篇
  2001年   3275篇
  2000年   4183篇
  1999年   1767篇
  1998年   619篇
  1997年   590篇
  1996年   543篇
  1995年   597篇
  1994年   586篇
  1993年   516篇
  1992年   482篇
  1991年   457篇
  1990年   483篇
  1989年   456篇
  1988年   592篇
  1987年   577篇
  1986年   437篇
  1985年   530篇
  1984年   437篇
  1983年   454篇
  1982年   366篇
  1981年   352篇
  1958年   641篇
  1957年   523篇
  1956年   448篇
  1955年   425篇
  1954年   334篇
  1953年   304篇
  1952年   292篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
761.
当前国内外运动心理学工作者重视和加强运动意识问题的研究。因为这是一个比较复杂的问题,迄今在我国有关运动心理学的教科书和文献资料中,对此没还有一个统一明确的概念和确切的阐述,本文仅就“什么是运动意识?”以及“运动意识的特征和作用”等问题,谈一点看法.一、什么是运动意识?人的任何意识都是在对周围客观事物的感知觉基础上,经过思维加工而形成的心理活动的最高级形式,并通过自己的语言和行动表现出来。意识也  相似文献   
762.
学习兴趣是推动学生探索知识,带有积极情绪色彩的一种意识倾向性。激发和培养学生的学习兴趣,对于巩固学生学习动机,调动学习积极性,提高学习效果和思想品德修养十分重要。学习兴趣是教育心理学研究课题之一。本文是对初中学生对各学科学习兴趣的问卷调查.对象是无锡市五所非重点中学初中一、二、三年  相似文献   
763.
Transfer of matching-to-figure samples in the pigeon   总被引:2,自引:1,他引:1       下载免费PDF全文
Three pigeons were trained on a modified six-key matching-to-sample procedure. The third peck on the figure-sample key (which presented a bird, hand, face, beetle, rabbit, fish, flower, or red hue, as the sample) lighted only one comparison key. Every three additional pecks on the sample lighted another comparison key, up to a maximum of five keys. Pecks on keys of matching figures produced grain. Pecks on nonmatching keys (mismatches) turned off all lights on the comparison keys and repeated the trial. Three figures were used during acquisition. The birds learned to peck each sample until the matching comparison stimulus appeared on one of three comparison stimulus keys, and then to peck that key. Later, five novel stimuli, employed as both sample and comparison stimuli, and two additional matching keys were added. Each bird showed matching transfer to the novel samples. The data suggest that the birds may have learned the concept of figure matching rather than a series of two-component chains or discrete five-key discriminations.  相似文献   
764.
Rats' lever pressing produced sucrose reinforcers on a variable-interval schedule where, in different conditions, the duration of a stimulus presented immediately after reinforcement was either correlated or uncorrelated with the duration of the current interreinforcement interval. Under the baseline schedule, in which no stimulus was presented, the minimum interreinforcement interval was 8 s and the mean postreinforcement pause of each subject approximated this value. Response rates increased slowly over the first 10 to 15 s and then remained roughly constant throughout the remainder of the interval. In both the correlated and uncorrelated conditions, the added stimulus resulted in the postreinforcement pauses lengthening to values in excess of the duration of the preceding stimulus. This resulted in a poststimulus pause which was, in most cases, roughly constant irrespective of the duration of the preceding stimulus, or of the reinforcement contingencies prevailing immediately after stimulus offset. Local response-rate patterns in the uncorrelated conditions were similar to those obtained under the baseline schedule in which no stimulus was presented. However, in the correlated condition local response rates increased across the remainder of the interreinforcer interval. Further, the rate of acceleration was inversely related to the duration of the preceding stimulus. These results show that a correlation between stimulus duration and the ensuing time to reinforcement can control behavior—a type of temporal control not previously reported.  相似文献   
765.
In order to investigate the development of movement speed in relation to movement organization, children of 5, 6, 7, 8 and 9 years of age and adults carried out a reciprocal tapping task, in which time pressure and distance were manipulated. The duration, velocity, acceleration and accuracy of the movements were compared between age groups. Age differences appeared mainly in the homing time, not in the duration of the distance covering movement phase. Accuracy and velocity of the distance covering movement phase differed with age. Time pressure affected the homing time, but not the duration of the distance covering phase. Distance manipulation affected mainly the velocity and duration of the distance covering movement phase and the homing time. In the discussion it is contended that age differences in homing time may be related to both the accuracy of the distance covering movement phase and the rate of information processing of the subject.  相似文献   
766.
767.
Studies were made of rapid error correction movements in eight subjects performing a visually guided tracking task involving flexion-extension movements about the elbow. Subjects were required to minimize reaction times in this two-choice task. Errors in initial movement direction occurred in about 3% of the trials. Error correction times (time from initiation to reversal of movement in incorrect direction) ranged from 30-150 ms. The first sing of correction of the error movement was a suppression of the electromyographic (EMG) activity in the muscle producing the error movement. This suppression started as early as 20-40 ms after the initiation of the error-related EMG activity and as much as 50 ms before any overt sign of limb movement. The correction of the error movement was also accompanied by an increase in the drive to the muscle which moved the arm in the correct direction. This increased activity always occurred after the initiation of the error movement. it is concluded that the first step in the error correction, suppression of drive to the muscle producing the error movement, cannot be based on information from the moving limb. It is thus suggested that this earliest response to the error movement is based on central monitoring of the commands for movement.  相似文献   
768.
We investigated the programming of generalization and maintenance of correspondence between verbal and nonverbal behavior in a preschool setting. Four children participated in a series of multiple-baseline designs. In Experiment 1, delayed reinforcement of verbal behavior effectively controlled maintenance of correspondence with previously trained responses and also resulted in generalization of correspondence to one untrained response. As the latter effect was limited, Experiment 2 was a further assessment of the effects of delayed reinforcement of generalization of correspondence to untrained responses, and consistent generalization was shown. Experiment 2 also showed that generalization, if lost, could be recovered through use of "booster training," in which the original contingencies were reinstated for a brief period. Experiment 3 provided replications, with two additional children, of the effects of delayed reinforcement on maintenance of correspondence. Results are discussed in terms of using delayed reinforcement as an indiscriminable contingency.  相似文献   
769.
770.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号