首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   11409篇
  免费   305篇
  国内免费   78篇
  2019年   22篇
  2018年   25篇
  2014年   26篇
  2012年   1241篇
  2011年   1310篇
  2010年   338篇
  2009年   148篇
  2008年   1057篇
  2007年   1074篇
  2006年   975篇
  2005年   825篇
  2004年   672篇
  2003年   683篇
  2002年   568篇
  2001年   445篇
  2000年   512篇
  1999年   206篇
  1998年   55篇
  1997年   53篇
  1996年   39篇
  1995年   36篇
  1994年   48篇
  1993年   50篇
  1992年   42篇
  1991年   47篇
  1990年   52篇
  1989年   47篇
  1988年   59篇
  1987年   44篇
  1986年   52篇
  1985年   52篇
  1984年   36篇
  1983年   42篇
  1982年   27篇
  1981年   42篇
  1980年   21篇
  1979年   28篇
  1978年   32篇
  1965年   23篇
  1963年   23篇
  1959年   28篇
  1958年   80篇
  1957年   74篇
  1956年   64篇
  1955年   46篇
  1954年   46篇
  1953年   49篇
  1952年   39篇
  1951年   24篇
  1950年   23篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
Orienting the finger opposition space during prehension movements   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
Two experiments are reported that examined the act of prehension when subjects were asked to grasp with their thumb and index finger pads an elongated object resting horizontally on a surface and placed at different orientations with respect to the subject. In Experiment 1, the pad opposition preferences were determined for the six angles of orientation examined. For angles of 90 degrees (object parallel to frontal plane) or less, no rotation of the wrist (pronation) was used; for angles 110 degrees or greater, pronation was systematically employed to reorient the finger opposition space. Only one angle, 100 degrees , produced any evidence of ambiguity in how to grasp the object: Approximately 60% of these grasps involved pronation and 40% did not. Using the foregoing grasp preference data, in Experiment 2 we examined the kinematics of the wrist and elbow trajectories during prehension movements directed at an object in different orientations. Movement time, time to peak acceleration, velocity, and deceleration were measured. No kinematic differences were observed when the object orientation either required (110 degrees ) or did not require (80 degrees ) a pronation. By contrast, if the orientation was changed at the onset of the movement, such that an unpredicted pronation had to be introduced to achieve the grasp, kinematics were affected: Movement time was increased, and the time devoted to deceleration was lengthened. These data are interpreted as evidence that when natural prehension occurs, pronation can be included in the motor plan without affecting the movement kinematics. When constraints are imposed on the movement execution as a consequence of a perturbation, however, the introduction of a pronation component requires kinematic rearrangement.  相似文献   
102.
我军医学论文产出的概况及简要分析第三军医大学(630038)王关嵩,罗长坤,刘久成,吴培红科学技术论文是研究与发展(R&D)活动主要产出形式之一。对发表论文的全面分析,可以从一个侧面了解学科的发展趋势、新兴领域的潜力、科技水平、科技人员的状况等。因此...  相似文献   
103.
卢卡奇的批判思想   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
G·卢卡奇是20世纪最权威的和最具有独创性的马克思主义思想家之一。说他是一个真正意义上权威的马克思主义者是因为,他的理论是以实践为导向或面向实践的,而且致力于改变世界。当然,这一理论包含着一个内容丰富的理论中介系统——从确立最抽象的方法论和人类学的基础到勾画彻底重建个体社会和共同体的具体方案。但是,对一种理论来说,以实践为导向,首先意味着它必须是批判性的。社会结构、政治制度、社会行为方式、文化领域,都不应被视为可孤立地思考、公正地描述、不考虑价值判断的分析和解释,或可作纯粹理解的既定对象。整个社会领域,包括已经被改造成为我们最接近的环境的那部分自然界,都必须看作是一种人的产品。因此,作为历史的一个阶段,它是满足且也破坏人类渴望的一项未完成的工作。对于任何有意义的人类未来来说,这些生活方式都是不可缺少的。与此同时,它们也有着显著的局限性,即削弱人类所付出的巨大努力,给人类带来连续不断的痛苦和苦难,因此是有待克服的。在辩证的扬弃,(Aufhebung)的这个意义上说,卢卡奇是一位第一流的批判思想家。  相似文献   
104.
维特根斯坦在第一次世界大战的战壕中写完他的第一本书——《逻辑哲学论》,当时他在奥地利军队中服役.一系列精雕细刻的格言由一个复杂的排列体系联结起来,并且一切都按照从形而上学到形式逻辑的方向排列.此书被罗素誉为是一部杰作.人们很快就广泛认识到这本书的重要性,特别是通过石里克、卡纳普和魏斯曼,维也纳学派认识到了它的重要性.《逻辑哲学论》立刻使维特根斯坦在哲学家的神殿中占有了一席之地.《逻辑哲学论》是维特根斯坦自己出版的唯一的一本书.有一段时间他放弃哲学去当了一名小学教师,后来又在奥地利做一名建筑师,1929年他回到剑桥,在那里断断续续地工作一直到他1951年去世.他的思想交流实际上仅局限于课堂,他在由学生和同事组成的非常亲密的  相似文献   
105.
106.
107.
108.
109.
110.
When subjects are required to produce short sequences of equally paced finger taps and to accentuate one of the taps, the interval preceding the forceful tap is shortened and the one that immediately follows the accent is lengthened. Assuming that the tapping movements are triggered by an internal clock, one explanation attributes the rnistiming of the taps to central factors: The momentary rate of the clock is accelerated or decelerated as a function of motor preparation to, respectively, increase or decrease the movement force. This hypothesis predicts that the interresponse intervals measured between either tap movement onsets or movement terminations (taps) will show the same timing pattern. A second explanation for the observed interval effects is that the tapping movements are triggered by a regular internal clock but the timing of the successive taps is altered because the forceful movement is completed in less time than the other tap movements are. This "peripheral" hypothesis predicts regular timing of movement onsets but distorted timing of movement terminations. In the present study, the trajectories of the movements performed by subjects were recorded and the interresponse intervals were measured at the beginning and the end of the tapping movements. The results of Experiment 1 showed that neither model can fully explain the interval effects: The fast forceful movements were initiated with an additional delay that took into account the small execution time of these movements. Experiment 2 reproduced this finding and showed that the timing of the onset and contact intervals did not evolve with the repetition of trial blocks. Therefore, the assumption of an internal clock that would trigger the successive movements must be rejected. The results are discussed in the framework of a modified two-stage model in which the internal clock, instead of triggering the tapping movements, provides target time points at which the movements have to produce their meaningful effects, that is, contacts with the response key. The timing distortions are likely to reflect both peripheral and central components.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号