首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   8135篇
  免费   145篇
  国内免费   9篇
  8289篇
  2012年   953篇
  2011年   1049篇
  2010年   243篇
  2009年   126篇
  2008年   785篇
  2007年   768篇
  2006年   747篇
  2005年   551篇
  2004年   540篇
  2003年   502篇
  2002年   418篇
  2001年   289篇
  2000年   372篇
  1999年   170篇
  1998年   38篇
  1997年   56篇
  1996年   20篇
  1995年   28篇
  1994年   15篇
  1993年   16篇
  1992年   19篇
  1991年   26篇
  1990年   15篇
  1989年   14篇
  1988年   7篇
  1987年   15篇
  1986年   11篇
  1985年   13篇
  1984年   16篇
  1983年   12篇
  1982年   16篇
  1980年   7篇
  1979年   11篇
  1978年   14篇
  1977年   7篇
  1975年   7篇
  1973年   9篇
  1964年   12篇
  1959年   17篇
  1958年   45篇
  1957年   43篇
  1956年   30篇
  1955年   34篇
  1954年   20篇
  1953年   22篇
  1952年   27篇
  1951年   25篇
  1950年   22篇
  1949年   19篇
  1948年   10篇
排序方式: 共有8289条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
121.
在1972年《命名与必然性》的演讲中,作者提出了直接指称论的观点:名字的语义学内容仅仅就是所指称的对象。一般认为,如果接受直接指称论就必然导致信念之谜。因为,假如专名的语义学内容仅仅就是所指称的对象,那么两个共指称的专名,比如"Cicero"("西塞罗")和"Tully"("图利"),就应该在信念语境中可以替换。但这似乎是错误的,因为,似乎"琼斯相信西塞罗是秃子但不相信图利是秃子"可以是真的,而倘若作替换,就会得出矛盾。在本文中,作者试图论证:实际上导致信念之谜的乃是两个更为基本的原则,即去引号原则和翻译原则;不能把信念之谜的产生归咎于直接指称论。通过这种间接论证,作者认为,直接指称论没有被信念之谜难题驳倒,它还是能够获得辩护的。  相似文献   
122.
According to the automatic response activation hypothesis of the dimensional overlap (DO) model (Kornblum, Stevens, Whipple, & Requin, 1999), stimulus-response compatibility effects are expected to occur in go-no-go tasks. This prediction is confirmed in two experiments in which subjects moved a hand to one side of the field on presentation of a go stimulus. Although the direction of movement was known in advance and the spatial attribute of the go stimuli was irrelevant to the go-no-go decision, the subjects' response time was shorter when the spatial attribute to the go stimulus corresponded to that of the response than when it did not. These effects are shown to depend on the similarity of the go and the no-go stimuli, as well as on whether the spatial attribute of the go stimuli was its actual location or its meaning. We discuss these results in terms of the temporal dynamics of automatic and controlled response processes, as hypothesized in the DO model.  相似文献   
123.
We studied the ability to localize flashed stimuli, using a relative judgment task. When observers are asked to localize the peripheral position of a probe with respect to the midposition of a spatially extended comparison stimulus, they tend to judge the probe as being more toward the periphery than is the midposition of the comparison stimulus. We report seven experiments in which this novel phenomenon was explored. They reveal that the mislocalization occurs only when the probe and the comparison stimulus are presented in succession, independent of whether the probe or the comparison stimulus comes first (Experiment 1). The size of the mislocalization is dependent on the stimulus onset asynchrony (Experiment 2) and on the eccentricity of presentation (Experiment 3). In addition, the illusion also occurs in an absolute judgment task, which links mislocalization with the general tendency to judge peripherally presented stimuli as being more foveal than they actually are (Experiment 4). The last three experiments reveal that relative mislocalization is affected by the amount of spatial extension of the comparison stimulus (Experiment 5) and by its structure (Experiments 6 and 7). This pattern of results allows us to evaluate possible explanations of the illusion and to relate it to comparable tendencies observed in eye movement behavior. It is concluded that the system in charge of the guidance of saccadic eye movements is also the system that provides the metric in perceived visual space.  相似文献   
124.
The speeded categorisation of gender from photographs of men's and women's faces under conditions of vertical brow and vertical head movement was explored in two sets of experiments. These studies were guided by the suggestion that a simple cue to gender in faces, the vertical distance between the eyelid and brow, could support such decisions. In men this distance is smaller than in women, and can be further reduced by lowering the brows and also by lowering the head and raising the eyes to camera. How does the gender-classification mechanism take changes in pose into account? Male faces with lowered brows (experiment 1) were more quickly and accurately categorised (there was little corresponding 'feminisation' of raised-brow faces). Lowering gaze had a similar effect, but failed to interact with head lowering in a simple manner (experiment 2). We conclude that the initial classification of gender from the facial image may not involve normalisation of the face image to a canonical state (the 'mug-shot view') for expressive pose (brow movement and direction of gaze). For head pose (relative position of the features when the face is not viewed head-on), normalisation cannot be ruled out. Some perceptual mechanisms for these effects, and their functional implications, are discussed.  相似文献   
125.
Since April 1996, the National Science and Technology Council (NSTC), in collaboration with the Office of Science and Technology Policy (OSTP) in the Executive Office of the President, has been leading the development of a government-wide Federal policy for research misconduct. The author is a Senior Policy Analyst in the Office of Science and Technology Policy and a participant in this process. This paper places the NSTC/OSTP effort in historical context, outlines the process by which the policy will be finalized, and discusses the principal issues raised during the policy development process.  相似文献   
126.
The minimum duration signal necessary to identify a set of spoken words was established by the gating technique; most words could be identified before their acoustic offset. Gated words were used as congruous and incongruous sentence completions, and isolation points established in the gating experiment were compared with the time course of semantic integration evident in event-related brain potentials. Differential N400 responses to contextually appropriate and inappropriate words were observed about 200 ms before the isolation point. Semantic processing was evident before the acoustic signal was sufficient to identify the words uniquely. Results indicate that semantic integration can begin to operate with only partial, incomplete information about word identity. Influences of semantic constraint, word frequency, and rate of presentation are described.  相似文献   
127.
全人类有什么普遍之处?怎样把他们区别开?在考察这两个问题时,可以根据类型分成三个阶段:柏拉图式的论述、浪漫主义的对比法和当代的综合,最后是在当今的人类生物学和语言学中被推进和发展了的假说。一、柏拉图式的论述柏拉图式的论述中经常被引证的章句,不是见于柏拉图而是他的学生亚里士多德的著作中。在一本早期的小册子《论解释》的开头这样写道:全人类的书写标记是不相同的,他们的有声语言也是不相同的。但它们首先都是灵魂的情感符号,在这一点上全都是一样的;这些情感具有的事物相似性也是相同的。这说明人们所关心的都是相…  相似文献   
128.
历史哲学看来是那些教小说的人的事。──弗兰克·科莫德要是在1950年,科莫德的观点一定会被当作奇谈怪论,在1968年时,它仍属前卫的观点,到1975年,在英语国家的历史哲学界,这种观点引起的问题已成了争论的最前沿问题。我将努力说明这种现象是怎样发生的,是在什么制度框架内发生的。直到第二次世界大战结束,相当程度上甚至直到今天,历史哲学几乎在任何地方都是一个学术孤儿。习惯上,它被视为是“柔弱的”,仅仅是一块可要可不要的研究领域。(软绵绵的恰恰是伦理学和美学,但很少有哲学系没有伦理学家和艺术哲学家,相反,确实没有…  相似文献   
129.
In 3 experiments, the authors examined movement space-time variability as a function of the force-time properties of the initial impulse in a movement timing task. In the range of motion and movement time task conditions, peak force, initial rate of force, and force duration were manipulated either independently or in combination across a range of parameter values. The findings showed that (a) impulse variability is predicted well by the elaboration of the isometric force variability scaling functions of L. G. Carlton, K. H. Kim, Y. T. Liu, and K. M. Newell (1993) to movement, and (b) the movement spatial and temporal outcome variability are complementary and well predicted by an equation treating the variance of force and time in Newton's 2nd law as independent random variables. Collectively, the findings suggest that movement outcome variability is the product of a coherent space-time function that is driven by the nonlinear scaling of the force-time properties of the initial impulse.  相似文献   
130.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号