首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1621篇
  免费   32篇
  国内免费   31篇
  2023年   9篇
  2022年   9篇
  2021年   8篇
  2019年   8篇
  2018年   17篇
  2014年   14篇
  2013年   10篇
  2012年   123篇
  2011年   133篇
  2010年   58篇
  2009年   23篇
  2008年   126篇
  2007年   133篇
  2006年   103篇
  2005年   94篇
  2004年   62篇
  2003年   104篇
  2002年   52篇
  2001年   50篇
  2000年   50篇
  1999年   28篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   12篇
  1995年   13篇
  1994年   22篇
  1993年   11篇
  1992年   14篇
  1991年   25篇
  1990年   25篇
  1989年   33篇
  1988年   37篇
  1987年   26篇
  1986年   28篇
  1985年   23篇
  1984年   9篇
  1983年   20篇
  1982年   12篇
  1981年   19篇
  1978年   11篇
  1965年   15篇
  1964年   8篇
  1963年   13篇
  1962年   5篇
  1958年   17篇
  1957年   11篇
  1956年   8篇
  1954年   3篇
  1953年   5篇
  1949年   3篇
排序方式: 共有1684条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
The prepared reflex (PR) metaphor (Exner, 1879; Woodworth, 1938) suggests that stimulus-response (S-R) instructions held in working memory (WM) can lead to autonomous response activation without any practice. Cohen-Kdoshay and Meiran (2007) showed flanker compatibility effects immediately following the instructions (First Trials Flanker Compatibility Effect, FTFCE) and also showed that FTFCE was eliminated when participants had to hold an additional novel task rule in mind. They attributed the elimination of the FTFCE to WM load, but did not rule out multitasking and associated increased control demands as a possible alternative explanation. In the present experiment, the authors compared a no-load condition, a load condition involving a secondary task that was changed in every block (thus requiring WM) and a multi-tasking condition involving a secondary that remained the same throughout the experiment. The results show FTFCE without load and in the multi-tasking condition but no FTFCE in the WM load condition, establishing the critical involvement of WM storage capacity in the FTFCE.  相似文献   
132.
Treinen E  Corneille O  Luypaert G 《Cognition》2012,122(2):247-251
Recent studies showed that stimuli are evaluated more favourably when they are perceived to capture others' attention, an effect coined "mimetic desire". The aim of the present research was to examine the combined role of Need for Cognition and target's facial trustworthiness in this effect. Participants saw movie excerpts of trustworthy and untrustworthy 3D faces (Oosterhof & Todorov, 2008) turning their attention towards one art painting and away from another art painting. Results showed that looked-at paintings were preferred to looked-away paintings when associated with a trustworthy face. However, the reversed finding was observed for paintings associated with untrustworthy faces. The latter interaction was in turn moderated by participant's Need for Cognition, with a larger reversal effect for participants scoring lower on the NFC scale. Theoretical implications of these findings are discussed.  相似文献   
133.
Two recent reviews of the neuroimaging literature on creativity have pointed to inconsistent findings across studies, calling into question the usefulness of the theoretical constructs motivating the search for its neural bases. However, it is argued that consistent patterns of neural activation do emerge when the cognitive process and the neuroimaging method are kept uniform across studies. To demonstrate this empirically, the activation likelihood estimation (ALE) method was used to conduct quantitative meta-analyses of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments of analogy and metaphor - two processes related to creativity and included in the recent reviews. The results demonstrated that analogy and metaphor reliably activate consistent but dissociable brain regions across fMRI studies. The implications of the findings for cognitive theories of analogy and metaphor are discussed. Furthermore, these results demonstrate that to the extent that creativity has heterogeneous sources, its neural instantiation will vary as a function of the underlying cognitive processes.  相似文献   
134.
The aim of the present study was to analyze if the left hemisphere preferentially controls flexion responses toward positive stimuli, while the right hemisphere is specialized toward extensor responses to negative pictures. To this end, right-handed subjects had to pull or push a joystick subsequent to seeing a positive or a negative stimulus in their left or right hemifield. Flexion responses were faster for positive stimuli, while negative stimuli were associated with faster extensions responses. Overall, performance was fastest when emotional stimuli were presented to the left visual hemifield. This right hemisphere superiority was especially clear for negative stimuli, while reaction times toward positive pictures showed no hemispheric difference. We did not find any interaction between hemifield and response type. Neither was there a triple interaction between valence, hemifield and response type. We suppose that response dichotomies in humans are not as tightly linked to a hemisphere- and valence-bound reaction type as previously assumed.  相似文献   
135.
To increase understanding of post-victimization symptom development, the present study investigated the role of shame- and guilt-proneness and event-related shame and guilt as potential risk factors. 35 individuals (M age = 31.7 yr.; 48.5% women), recently victimized by a single event of severe violent crime, were assessed regarding shame- and guilt-proneness, event-related shame and guilt, and post-victimization symptoms. The mediating role of event-related shame was investigated with structural equation modeling (SEM), using bootstrapping. The guilt measures were unrelated to each other and to post-victimization symptoms. The shame measures were highly intercorrelated and were both positively correlated to more severe post-victimization symptom levels. Event-related shame as mediator between shame-proneness and post-victimization symptoms was demonstrated by prevalent significant indirect effects. Both shame measures are potent risk factors for distress after victimization, whereby part of the effect of shame-proneness on post-victimization symptoms is explained by event-related shame.  相似文献   
136.
在浩如繁星的文学流派中,公教文学以其独具魅力在历史上占有特殊的地位。虽然,在人们的印象中,它传承的是希伯来文化和希腊文化的人文与道德观,成为西方文化的重要组成部分,但在其传播过程中,它却对世界人类文明的发展进程起到了举足轻重的作用。  相似文献   
137.
正思维——在表象、概念的基础上,进行分析、推理、判断,以爱综合、归纳等一系列认识活动的过程。这是给思维下的定义。思维是人类独有的精神活动、意识动态。因为,唯人才有灵魂,灵魂是思维的根源,而思维则是灵魂的属性与功能。其他的生命体比如:飞禽走兽、牛马猪羊,即便有大脑也只能"跟着感觉走"。盖因大脑只不过是灵魂借以思维的工具而已。其本身并不能产生思维。不然,猪羊闻讯早遛之大吉,逃之夭夭了,还轮到你屠夫"磨刀霍霍  相似文献   
138.
党的十八大以来,以习近平同志为核心的党中央提出一系列关于宗教工作的新理念新举措,回答了新时代怎样认识宗教、怎样处理宗教问题、怎样做好党的宗教工作等重大理论和实践问题。这一系列新理念新举措可以概括为“九个必须”。  相似文献   
139.
140.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号